Dunn, Wilson, & Gilbert (2003)
pdf
keyboard_arrow_up
School
Simon Fraser University *
*We aren’t endorsed by this school
Course
SOCIAL PSY
Subject
Psychology
Date
Oct 30, 2023
Type
Pages
12
Uploaded by mkd16
10.1177/0146167203256867
ARTICLE
PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
Dunn et al. / MISPREDICTING SATISFACTION
Location, Location, Location: The Misprediction
of Satisfaction in Housing Lotteries
Elizabeth W. Dunn
Timothy D. Wilson
University of Virginia
Daniel T. Gilbert
Harvard University
People tend to overestimate the emotional consequences of future
life events, exhibiting an impact bias. The authors replicated the
impact bias in a real-life context in which undergraduates were
randomly assigned to dormitories (or “houses”). Participants
appeared to focus on the wrong factors when imagining their
future happiness in the houses. They placed far greater weight on
highly variable physical features than on less variable social fea-
tures in predicting their future happiness in each house, despite
accurately recognizing that social features were more important
than physical features when asked explicitly about the determi-
nants of happiness. In Experiment 2, we found that this discrep-
ancy emerged in part because participants exhibited an isolation
effect, focusing too much on factors that distinguished between
houses and not enough on factors that varied only slightly, such
as social features.
Keywords:
affective forecasting; impact bias; predictors of happiness;
emotion; isolation effect
I
t is safe to say that chocolate cake, sunny spring days,
and true love are more likely to produce happiness than
are mud pies, freezing rain, and bad breakups. Yet, even
if people accurately recognize what factors will lead to
happiness, they may fail to apply this knowledge in imag-
ining how they will feel in the future. For example, con-
sider the case of a high school senior touring two univer-
sities that differ greatly in terms of setting (urban vs.
rural), size, and gender ratios but are very similar in
terms of dorm life, available extracurriculars, and oppor-
tunities for contact with professors. In predicting how
happy he or she would be at each university, the rational
student should weight the quality of each aspect (e.g.,
availability of extracurriculars) by its importance to his
or her happiness. Yet, we would argue that the student
may inadvertently place undue weight on those factors
that vary a great deal across universities while placing lit-
tle or no weight on important factors that vary less across
options in imagining his or her future well-being at each
school.
This prediction follows from Tversky’s (1972) elimi-
nation-by-aspects theory of choice. According to the the-
ory, people simplify choices between options by cancel-
ing out and disregarding features that are shared across
options, a tendency Kahneman and Tversky (1979) term
the “isolation effect” (see also Houston & Sherman,
1995; Houston, Sherman, & Baker, 1991). Applying this
theory, Hodges (1997) asked participants to choose
between three apartments, including two that shared
several very positive features but also had unique nega-
tive features, as well as one that had a unique set of posi-
tive and negative features. Although the two apartments
with shared features were more attractive, on balance,
than the third apartment, participants exhibited a pref-
erence for the latter because they cancelled out the posi-
tive, shared features of the first two apartments, paying
attention primarily to the unique, negative features.
1421
Authors’ Note:
This research was supported in part by a National Sci-
ence Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship to the first author and
Grant R01-MH56075 from the National Institute of Mental Health to
the second and third authors. We thank Jack McArdle for statistical ad-
vice and Kevin Carlsmith, Gerald Clore, and Brian Malone for com-
ments on an earlier draft. The article is based on the first author’s
master’s thesis project. Portions of these results were presented at the
2001 meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, San
Antonio, Texas. Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Elizabeth W. Dunn, Department of Psychology, University of
Virginia, 102 Gilmer Hall, P.O. Box 400400, Charlottesville, VA 22904-
4400; e-mail: edunn@post.harvard.edu.
PSPB,
Vol. 29 No. 11, November 2003
1421-1432
DOI: 10.1177/0146167203256867
© 2003 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.
Focusingonunique featureswhile cancelingoutshared
features may be a reasonable and efficient strategy for
choosing between options. For example, it would be sen-
sible for the college-bound student to pay attention pri-
marily to features that varied a great deal across Univer-
sity A and University B in deciding which school he or
she would rather attend. But it would surely be unwise
for the student to conclude that he or she would be bliss-
fully happy at University A and miserable at University B
while neglecting important factors that were similar
across these universities.
To the extent that people overestimate differences in
the emotional consequences of various outcomes due to
ignoring shared features, they are likely to enjoy desir-
able outcomes less than they expect while reaping unan-
ticipated happiness from less desirable outcomes. This
tendency may help to explain why individuals are often
woefully inaccurate in predicting how various outcomes
will affect their happiness (Buehler & McFarland, 2001;
Gilbert, Driver-Linn, & Wilson, 2002; Gilbert, Lieberman, &
Wilson, in press; Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson, Blumberg, &
Wheatley, 1998; Gilbert & Wilson, 2000; Loewenstein &
Schkade, 1999; Mitchell, Thompson, Peterson, & Cronk,
1997; Sieff, Dawes, & Loewenstein, 1999; Wilson & Gilbert,
in press; Wilson, Wheatley, Meyers, Gilbert, & Axsom,
2000). People frequently overestimate the duration and
intensity of their own emotional responses to future
events and outcomes, a tendency that Gilbert et al.
(2002) term the “impact bias.” In everyday life, people
often engage in affective forecasting when envisioning
competing possibilities (e.g., receiving tenure vs. not, liv-
ing in the suburbs vs. the city, ordering foie gras vs. frog
legs). As an important artifact of this process, features
that differ between competing options may drive fore-
casts, whereas features that are relatively constant take a
backseat.
In a similar vein, Wilson et al. (2000) showed thatindi-
viduals exhibit the impact bias in part because they over-
look the fact that the emotional impact of any one event
will be mitigated by other events, such as snowstorms,
parties, and the other ups and downs of daily life, an
oversight Wilson et al. (2000) termed “focalism” (see
also Schkade & Kahneman, 1998). We suggest that the
isolation effect may produce a different kind of focusing
bias, in which individuals ignore the affective conse-
quences of important features that are shared across
potential outcomes, leading to exaggerated expecta-
tions regarding differences in future well-being associ-
ated with one outcome versus another.
The housing system at a major university provided us
with an opportunity to conduct an initial test of this
hypothesis. In the spring of their 1st year, students at the
university are randomly assigned to spend the subse-
quent 3 years of college living in 1 of 12 dormitories (or
“houses”), with each serving as a kind of college within
the college. Each 1st-year student enters the housing lot-
tery along with the roommates he or she has chosen as
well as a group of up to 15 other friends (“blockmates”)
who are automatically assigned to the same house. Thus,
by the time students enter the housing lottery, they know
the group of people whom they will live with the follow-
ing year. In addition, most 1st-year students are familiar
with the location and other physical characteristics of
the houses as well as the individual “character” of each
house (e.g., one house is known for its spirited intramu-
ral teams, another for its parties, another for its historical
tolerance of gays and other stigmatized groups). The
houses vary a great deal in perceived quality, and most
students anticipate that housing assignment will strongly
influence their happiness; 1st-year students often stay up
all night awaiting their housing assignment and can be
seen jumping up and down or accepting consolation
from an upper-class student after receiving news that
they have been assigned to a desirable or undesirable
house. Thus, the housing system provides a natural
experiment in that students are randomly assigned to a
good or bad outcome that they consider extremely
important.
Whereas physical features (e.g., location) differ
greatly across houses, social aspects of house life (e.g.,
sense of community) vary less across the houses. Indeed,
because one enters the housing lottery with a group of
roommates and blockmates, the quality of one’s rela-
tionships with these close others should be roughly
equivalent regardless of house assignment. Typically,
social relationships are a critical determinant of happi-
ness (Argyle, 1999; Biswah-Diener & Diener, 2001;
Diener, Gohm, Suh, & Oishi, 2000; Diener, Suh, Lucas,
Smith, 1999; Larson, 1990; Myers, 1999; Sheldon, Elliot,
Kim, & Kasser, 2001); indeed, Diener and Seligman
(2002) speculated that perhaps “good social relation-
ships are, like food and thermoregulation, universally
important to human mood” (p. 83). When asked directly
about the determinants of happiness, most people seem
well aware of the critical role of close relationships
(Campbell, Converse, & Rodgers, 1976; Freedman,
1978; Loewenstein & Schkade, 1999; Pettijohn &
Pettijohn, 1996). For example, respondents at Pittsburgh
International airport rated family life and friends as
most important to happiness while rating income as less
important (Loewenstein, 1996, described in
Loewenstein & Schkade, 1999). Thus, we predicted that
social aspects of house life would be related to actual
happiness and thatparticipants would recognize the crit-
ical importance of social features when explicitly asked
about determinants of happiness.
Yet, we anticipated that when predicting their future
happiness, students would pay little attention to the
1422
PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
quality of social features that they expected to find in the
houses because social features varied less than physical
features across houses. In other words, we predicted that
the tendency to focus on highly variable features (i.e.,
theisolationeffect)wouldoverwhelmparticipants’explicit
recognition that less variable social features are critical
to happiness.
In two experiments, we examined whether partici-
pants overestimated how happy they would be living in a
desirable house as well as how unhappy they would be liv-
ing in an undesirable house, thereby exhibiting the
impact bias. To the best of our knowledge, this repre-
sents the first attempt to demonstrate the impact bias
using amultiyear longitudinal designwithrandom assign-
ment to a major life outcome. In addition, we attempted
to demonstrate a mechanism for the impact bias that has
not previously been investigated, namely, the isolation
effect.As discussed, we predicted thatparticipants would
underestimate the importance of features that the houses
had in common (social relationships), even though they
knew, in the abstract, that these features would be impor-
tant to their happiness. We predicted that participants
would overestimate the importance of features that var-
ied across houses (physical features).
EXPERIMENT 1
Method
Overview
. In the spring of their 1st year (Time 1),
shortly before they learned which house they would live
in for the subsequent 3 years, college students predicted
how happy they would be 1 year later if they were living in
each of 12 houses. One and 2 years later (Times 2 and 3),
we asked participants to report their actual happiness.
We predicted thatpeople would overestimate how happy
theywould be if theywere assigned a desirable house and
underestimate how happy they would be if they were
assigned an undesirable house. Furthermore, people
were expected to base their forecasts more on physical
features that varied greatly between houses and less on
social features that varied relatively little, even though
social features would be more related to actual happi-
ness than physical features.
Participants
. At Time 1, 174 1st-years completed our
initial questionnaire. Of these, 118 completed the Time
2 survey as sophomores and 84 completed the Time 3
survey as juniors.
Time 1 survey
. The initial survey asked participants to
predict how happy they would be overall at the same
time next year if they were living in each of the 12 houses
on a scale ranging from 1 (
unhappy
) to 7 (
happy
). Similar
one-item measures have been used successfully in other
affective forecasting studies (e.g., Gilbert et al., 1998)
and possess acceptable psychometric properties (Diener
et al., 1999; Fordyce, 1988). In addition, participants esti-
mated the extent to which 10 features related to house
life (e.g., location, sense of community) would influence
their happiness with the house on a scale ranging from 1
(
will have no influence on myhappiness
) to 7 (
will have a large
impact on my happiness
). Six of these features were physi-
cal qualities of the house, whereas four of the features
were related to the quality of social life in the house (see
Table 1). All participants completed the survey approxi-
mately 1 to 2 weeks before they were randomly assigned
to the house where they would spend their sophomore,
junior, and senior years.
Time 2 survey
. One year after completing the initial
survey, participants were asked to report their overall
happiness on the same 7-point scale used in the first sur-
vey.
1
Participants also reported how much each of the
physical and social features of house life had influenced
their happiness on a scale corresponding to that used at
Time 1. In addition, participants were instructed to rate
the objective quality of these 10 features of life in their
house on a7-point scale (1=
poor
,4=
average
,7=
excellent
).
Time 3 survey
. Two years after the initial survey, partici-
pants again reported their happiness and provided
objective ratings of the quality of the 10 features of house
life on the same scales used at Time 2. All surveys were
conducted via e-mail and were completed between
March 19 and March 27 of each year (except for two par-
ticipants who completed the Time 2 survey during the
first week of April).
Results and Discussion
Impact bias
. Across the sample as a whole, participants’
forecasted happiness was significantly (although not
highly) correlated with their overall happiness a year
later,
r
(109) = .26,
p
< .006. Fifty-three participants were
assigned to houses in which they expected to be below
their personal mean in predicted happiness (undesir-
able house group), whereas 56 participants were
assigned to houses where they expected to be at or above
their personal mean (desirable house group).
2
Partici-
pants who were assigned to houses they considered
undesirable were significantly happier overall (
M
= 5.38,
SD
= 1.16) at Time 2 than they had expected to be (
M
=
3.43,
SD
= 1.5),
t
(52) = 10.71,
p
< .0001,
d
= 1.49. In con-
trast, participants who were assigned to desirable houses
were significantly less happy overall (
M
= 5.45,
SD
= 0.92)
than they had expected (
M
= 5.96,
SD
= 0.85),
t
(55) =
–3.04,
p
< .01,
d
= 0.41. Consistent with previous research,
then, we found that participants overestimated how
much an important life event would affect their happi-
ness. Indeed, participants assigned to desirable houses
Dunn et al. / MISPREDICTING SATISFACTION
1423
Your preview ends here
Eager to read complete document? Join bartleby learn and gain access to the full version
- Access to all documents
- Unlimited textbook solutions
- 24/7 expert homework help
were not significantly happier than participants assigned
to undesirable houses,
t
< 1.
Features related to actual happiness
. To examine whether
forecasters go astray partly because they focus on the
wrong factors in imagining their emotional futures, we
first sought to demonstrate that the quality of social fea-
tures in the houses predicted participants’ actual happi-
ness. We summed participants’ Time 2 ratings of the
objective quality of the four social features in their house
and their ratings of the objective quality of the six physi-
cal features in their house to create social and physical
quality composites, which we centered at 0 by subtract-
ing the composite means. We entered the composite
quality of social features (
α
= .50) and the composite
quality of physical features (
α
= .55) into a regression
predicting happiness at Time 2; in this analysis and sub-
sequent analyses, we first examined the main effects of
social and physical quality in a simultaneous regression,
and then the interaction term was added to the model in
a second step. There was a marginally significant rela-
tionship between overall happiness and the quality of
social features,
t
(114) = 1.66,
β
= .16,
p
= .10, whereas
overall happiness was unrelated to the quality of physical
features,
t
(114) = 0.66,
β
= .06,
p
= .51, or to the interac-
tion of social and physical feature quality,
t
(113) = –1.24,
β
= –.12,
p
= .22.
We examined these relationships again the following
year using Time 3 ratings of physical and social features
to predict happiness at Time 3. The quality of social fea-
tures was again positively related to overall happiness,
t
(77) = 2.75,
β
= .35,
p
< .01. Unlike at Time 2, the quality
of physical features was negatively related to overall hap-
piness,
t
(77) = –2.27,
β
= –.29,
p
< .03.
3
The two-way inter-
action of physical and social features was nonsignificant,
t
(76) = –0.42,
β
= –.05,
p
= .67. Thus, across their sopho-
more and junior years, participants’ happiness was con-
sistently related to their ratings of social life in the house.
In contrast, the relationship between physical features
and happiness was nonsignificant or in the opposite to
the expected direction.
Features related to forecasted happiness
. Do forecasters
recognize the relationship between social features and
their future happiness? At Time 1, when asked directly
about the determinants of their happiness, forecasters
predicted that the four social features would have a
slightly greater impact on their happiness (
M
= 4.91,
SD
=
1.10) on average than would the six physical features
(
M
= 4.67,
SD
= 0.93),
t
(104) = 2.33,
p
< .02.
Yet, in imagining how happy they would be in specific
houses, forecasters might overlook the role of social fea-
tures. To explore this possibility, we examined the rela-
tionship between participants’ affective forecasts at
Time 1 and their ratings of the physical and social fea-
tures of their house at Time 2 (recall that participants
did not rate these features atTime 1).We entered Time 2
ratings of physical quality and social quality, followed by
the two-way interaction, into a regression predicting
forecasted happiness at Time 1 for the house to which
the participant was actually assigned. Ratings of the phys-
ical quality of the house at Time 2 were strongly related
to forecasted happiness,
t
(106) = 4.32,
β
= .39,
p
< .0005,
suggesting that participants were aware of the physical
characteristics of the houses and relied heavily on this
information in predicting their future happiness.Incon-
trast, ratings of social features at Time 2 were unrelated
to forecasted happiness,
t
(106) = 0.26,
β
= .02,
p
= .80,
suggesting that participants were unable to predict the
quality of social features they would experience in the
house or placed little weight on this information in fore-
casting their happiness. The two-way interaction was
nonsignificant,
t
(105) = –1.49,
β
= –.14,
p
= .14.
The overemphasis placed on physical features helps
to account for the discrepancy between participants’
affective forecasts and experiences. We entered partici-
pants’ Time 2 ratings of social and physical features into
a regression predicting the difference between actual
and forecasted happiness and then added the two-way
interaction to the model. Participants who reported
high satisfaction with the physical features of their house
exhibited relatively low overall happiness compared to
their forecasts,
t
(106) = –3.96,
β
= –.37,
p
< .0005, suggest-
ing that physical features did not deliver the degree of
happiness participantshadanticipated;neither the qual-
ity of social features,
t
(106) = 0.81,
β
= .08,
p
= .42, nor the
two-way interaction,
t
(105) = 0.34,
β
= .03,
p
= .74, signifi-
cantly predicted the difference between overall happi-
ness and predicted happiness.
Summary of results
. As expected, we found strong sup-
port for the impact bias; participants overestimated how
happy they would be in desirable houses and how miser-
able they would be in undesirable houses. Our results
suggest that forecasters may have erred by focusing on
physical features such as location while virtually ignoring
the quality of social life in the houses. When our
1424
PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
TABLE 1:
Physical and Social Features Relevant to House Life
Physical Features
Social Features
Location
Relationships with roommates
Attractiveness
Relationships with blockmates (friends
who enter housing lottery together)
Room size
Relationships with tutors (graduate/
faculty advisers who live or eat in the
house)
House size
Sense of community in the house
Facilities (e.g., for
athletics, arts, etc.)
Dining hall
participants were sophomores and juniors, their ratings
of house social life were reliably related to their actual
happiness, whereas their ratings of house physical fea-
tures did not consistently predict happiness. Yet, the
affective forecasts they made as 1st-years were strongly
related to the quality of physical features and unrelated
to the quality of social life they later found in the houses.
Of interest, participants accurately reported that social
features would strongly influence their happiness when
they were explicitly asked about the determinants of
happiness. Thus, although quality of social life was
related to happiness and participants recognized that
this would be the case, participants’ forecasts were unre-
lated to the quality of social life they found in the houses.
Instead, forecasts were driven by physical qualities of the
houses, which failed to deliver the lasting pleasure par-
ticipants anticipated.
Because the Time 1 survey did not ask participants to
predict the quality of social and physical features in all 12
houses, we could only examine the relationship between
Time 2 ratings of social and physical quality and Time 1
affective forecasts. It is possible, then, that participants
estimated the quality of social life they would find the fol-
lowing year and used these estimates in their affective
forecasts, but thattheir estimates were highly inaccurate,
thereby eliminating any relationship between forecasts
and the quality of social life they actually experienced.
To tackle this potential problem, we asked a new sample
of 1st-years to predict the quality of each of the social and
physical features they expected to find in each of the 12
houses.
In addition, we tested the isolation effect more
directly by asking some participants to think about fea-
tures that would be the same across houses immediately
before forecasting. We predicted that this manipulation
would reduce the isolation effect, thereby increasing the
weight participants placed on social features. Finally,
Study 2 addressed a potential alternative explanation of
the results of Study 1, namely, that there was insufficient
variance in people’s ratings of social features to allow for
correlations with predicted or actual happiness.
EXPERIMENT 2
Method
Time 1 survey
. We gave a longer version of the Time 1
survey used in the first experiment to a new sample of
144 1st-year participants at the same university, who com-
pleted it approximately 1 to 2 weeks before receiving
their housing assignment. In addition to predicting how
happy they would be 1 year later if they were living in
each of the 12 houses, participants predicted how much
the 10 features of house life examined in Study 1 would
influence their happiness. Next, participants rated the
objective quality of each of the physical and social fea-
tures they expected to find in each of the 12 houses. For
example, in considering a given house, they rated how
good the location would be and how good the sense of
community would be. They were asked to write “dk” or
“don’t know” if they felt they could not predict how good
a certain feature would be in a given house; otherwise, all
of the scales were equivalent to those used in the previ-
ous study. Finally, participants rated how much each of
the features would vary across different houses on a 7-
point scale (1 =
would not vary at all
, 7 =
would vary a great
deal
) as well as how easily they could predict the quality of
each feature (1 =
very difficult to predict
, 7 =
very easy to pre-
dict
) and how easily they could visualize each feature (1 =
very difficult to visualize
, 7 =
very easy to visualize
).
Manipulation of isolation effect
. Immediately prior to
completing the survey, a subset of participants was ran-
domly assigned to answer questions designed to manipu-
late their focus on features that varied relatively little
across houses; these participants were asked to write
about aspects of the houses and house life that would be
“pretty much the same” across houses immediately
before forecasting. To reduce potential demand charac-
teristics, we included a separate question that asked par-
ticipants to write about aspects that would “vary a great
deal” across houses. One group of participants (
N
= 28)
first wrote about what would vary and then about what
would be the same across houses (same-last condition).
We expected these participants to place increased
weight on social features because they thought about
aspects of house life that would be similar across houses
immediately before forecasting.
Another group of participants (
N
= 40) answered the
two questions in the reverse order (vary-last condition),
such that they thought about highly variable aspects of
house life immediately before forecasting. Control par-
ticipants (
N
= 76) were not asked any questions before
completing the main part of the survey. If participants
focused on highly variable features by default, then par-
ticipants in both the control and the vary-last conditions
should place greater weight on physical features than
social features in making affective forecasts, whereas par-
ticipants in the same-last condition should show a reduc-
tion in this bias.
Time 2 survey
. One year after completing the initial
survey, participants were asked to complete the same
questions used at Time 2 in Experiment 1; participants
rated their actual happiness as well as the affective
impact and objective quality of the various physical and
social features of their house. A total of 90 participants
returned the Time 2 survey (63% of original sample).
Dunn et al. / MISPREDICTING SATISFACTION
1425
Results and Discussion
Replication of Study 1
. As in Study 1, we found strong
support for the impact bias. Participants who were
assigned to houses they considered undesirable were sig-
nificantly happier overall (
M
= 5.0,
SD
= 1.11) than they
had expected to be (
M
= 3.82,
SD
= 0.86),
t
(35) = 5.26,
p
<
.001,
d
= 0.89, whereas participants who were assigned to
desirable houses were less happy (
M
= 5.41,
SD
= 1.35)
than they had anticipated (
M
= 6.02,
SD
= 0.94),
t
(51) =
2.81,
p
< .007,
d
= 0.39. Although participants in desirable
houses reported somewhat greater overall happiness
than participants in undesirable houses, this difference
did not reach significance,
t
(86) = 1.52,
p
= .13,
d
= 0.33.
Consistent with Study 1, there was a modest correlation
between forecasted happiness and actual happiness,
r
(88) = .19,
p
< .08.
Replicating Study 1, participants predicted that the
quality of social features in the house would influence
their happiness (
M
= 5.35,
SD
= 0.82) more than the qual-
ity of physical features would (
M
= 4.92,
SD
= 0.85) when
they were asked directly about determinants of happi-
ness,
t
(141) = 5.71,
p
< .0001. However, we predicted that
participants’ affective forecasts would actually be driven
by the quality of physical features in the house rather
than by the quality of social life participants anticipated.
For each house, we entered ratings of physical quality
(mean
α
= .81) and social quality (mean alpha = .84) into
a simultaneous regression predicting forecasted happi-
ness and then added the two-way interaction to the
model. In this way, we conducted 12 separate between-
subjects regressions (one for each house). Across the 12
houses, there were no significant positive relationships
between participants’ ratings of social features and their
forecasted happiness; for one house, there was a margin-
ally significant negative relationship between antici-
pated social quality and forecasted happiness (see Table
2).
4
In contrast, significant or marginally significant posi-
tive relationships emerged between ratings of physical
features and affective forecasts for 10 of the 12 houses.
The two-way interaction was nonsignificant for 11 of the
12 houses. In line with our hypotheses, then, partici-
pants placed a great deal of weight on physical features
of the houses and neglected social features entirely in
imagining their future happiness—despite recognizing
on an explicit level that social features mattered more
than physical features.
Manipulation of isolation effect
. We expected that asking
participants to consider features that would be similar
across houses would lead them to think about social fea-
tures, whereas asking participants to think about fea-
tures that would vary across houses would lead them to
think about physical features. As a check on this manipu-
lation, three research assistantswho were unaware of our
hypotheses coded participants’ responses to the ques-
tions regarding what would vary and what would be the
same across houses. For each question (vary and same),
the coders noted whether participants mentioned each
of the social and physical features of the houses. The
coders showed considerable agreement, with effective
reliabilities of .90, .88, .89, and .88 for ratings of the num-
ber of times participants mentioned physical features
under the vary question, social features under the vary
question, physical features under the same question,
and social features under the same question, respec-
tively. Averaging coders’ ratings, we found that partici-
pants mentioned physical features (
M
= 1.88,
SD
= 1) sig-
nificantly more than social features (
M
= 0.48,
SD
= 0.67)
when asked what would vary across houses,
t
(67) = 9.22,
p
< .001, and mentioned social features (
M
= 0.69,
SD
=
0.74) significantly more than physical features (
M
= 0.42,
SD
= 0.60) when asked what would be similar across
houses,
t
(67) = –2.36,
p
< .02, as expected.
To examine whether our manipulation influenced
the weight participants placed on social features, we
used a multilevel linear model, an approach thatallowed
us to include all of our data in a single powerful analysis
while accounting for the lack of independence between
a single individual’s ratings of the 12 houses (Bryk &
Raudenbush, 1992; Kashy & Kenny, 2000; Kenny, Kashy,
& Bolger, 1998). Although our model used continuous
predictors, this analysis is analogous to a between-within
ANOVA with repeated measures on one factor and a
between-subjects manipulation on another factor. Multi-
level models are increasingly used in repeated-measures
designs in which each participant provides ratings on the
dependent and independent variable at a series of time
points; in such models, time is the lower-level unit nested
1426
PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
TABLE 2:
Summary of Regression Analyses Predicting Forecasted
Happiness From Ratings of Social and Physical Qualities
for Each House
Social
Physical
Social
×
Quality
Quality
Physical
House
N
β
t
β
t
β
t
House 1
40
–.13
–.80
.64
3.96*** –.44
–2.79**
House 2
31
–.07
–.28
.53
2.02**
.09
0.49
House 3
30
–.24
–.73
.66
2.00*
.02
0.07
House 4
41
.10
.56
.66
3.66***
.19
1.37
House 5
35
–.07
–.22
.45
1.44
.40
1.92
House 6
32
–.26
–1.30
.66
3.30**
–.23
–1.39
House 7
30
–.58
–1.74* 1.13
3.36**
.31
1.81
House 8
36
–.11
–.41
.60
2.30**
.31
1.48
House 9
38
–.09
–.59
.82
5.09***
.18
1.30
House 10
28
–.29
–.90
.93
2.90**
.10
0.51
House 11
33
–.08
–.29
.70
2.54**
.03
0.19
House 12
30
.00
.00
.58
1.58
.34
1.70
*
p
≤
.10. **
p
≤
.05. ***
p
≤
.001.
Your preview ends here
Eager to read complete document? Join bartleby learn and gain access to the full version
- Access to all documents
- Unlimited textbook solutions
- 24/7 expert homework help
within person, the upper-level unit. Our data can be
thought of as similar to a repeated-measures design, with
each house providing a measurement point. In our
model, house is a lower-level unit nested within partici-
pant, the upper-level unit. Condition represents an
upper-level variable because the manipulation was at the
level of the participant. Affective forecasts (the depend-
ent variable) and ratings of physical and social quality
are lower-level variables measured at the house level.
We entered the upper-level variable of condition and
the lower-level variables of anticipated social quality and
anticipated physical quality, along with the interactions
of these three variables, into a multilevel linear model
predicting forecasted happiness (measured at the lower
level of house). In essence, this multilevel model (a) cre-
ated a regression equation predicting forecasted happi-
ness from physical and social quality ratings for each par-
ticipant across houses, (b) combined these equations to
create an aggregate, and (c) compared whether the
regression coefficients differed by condition. Inline with
our expectations, preliminary analyses revealed that the
vary-last condition did not differ from the control condi-
tion; thus, we collapsed across these two conditions,
which we contrasted with the same-last condition.
The multilevel model revealed a very strong positive
relationship between anticipated quality of physical
features and forecasted happiness,
t
(336) = 12.02,
B
=
0.23,
p
< .001, whereas the anticipated quality of social
features was unrelated to forecasted happiness,
t
(336) =
–0.70,
B
= –0.03,
p
= .49 (see Table 3).
5
The difference
between the weight placed on physical versus social fea-
tures may have been magnified in this analysis because
there was much greater variability in any one partici-
pant’s ratings of physical features (mean
SD
= 4.4) than
social features (mean
SD
= 1.76) across houses. Because
of this statistical artifact, the relative size of these main
effects should not be overinterpreted, but this finding is
at least consistent with the 12 between-subjects regres-
sions showing that participants’ affective forecasts were
driven by the quality of the house’s physical features
rather than by social features.
The only other significant effect was the expected
two-way Social Quality
×
Condition interaction,
t
(336) =
2.59,
B
= 0.08,
p
< .01. We plotted the mean forecasted
happiness predicted by the model at 1
SD
above and
below the mean social quality rating for participants in
the same-last condition and the control/vary-last condi-
tion based on standard guidelines (Aiken & West, 1991).
As seen in Figure 1, this interaction demonstrated that
participants placed greater weight on the quality of
social features if they were asked to think about features
of house life that would be similar across houses immedi-
ately before forecasting. This finding, we should note, is
inconsistent with the interpretation that there was too little
variance in ratings of social factors to allow for predict-
ability. People in the same-last condition, as expected,
did appear to place greater weight on social factors than
other participants when making their forecasts.
Although thismodel provides support for our hypoth-
eses, it is based on only 62 out of our 144 participants
(43%). Many participants had missing data for the social
and physical composite variables because they failed to
predict the quality of at least one of the house features,
most likely due to the fact that some of the features (e.g.,
tutor quality) were relatively difficult to predict (recall
that participants were given the option to say “don’t
know”). To compensate for this shortcoming, we con-
ducted an additional analysis in which we substituted
anticipated roommate quality for the social quality com-
posite and anticipated quality of location for the physical
quality composite. Roommate quality was rated as both
Dunn et al. / MISPREDICTING SATISFACTION
1427
TABLE 3:
Multilevel Model Predicting Forecasted Happiness From
Condition and Social and Physical Composites
B
df
t
Fixed effects
Physical quality
.23
336
12.02***
Social quality
–.03
336
–0.70
Condition
.14
60
1.38
Social Quality
×
Condition
.08
336
2.59**
Physical Quality
×
Condition
–.02
336
–0.94
Social Quality
×
Physical Quality
–.01
336
–1.30
Physical Quality
×
Social Quality
×
Condition
–.01
336
–1.60
Random effects
Intercept (individual differences
in forecasts)
.27
2.4**
Physical quality
.00
0.53
Social quality
.01
1.49
Residual
1.06
11.89***
NOTE: Total observations = 1,728; observations used = 404;
R
2
= .54.
*
p
≤
.10. **
p
≤
.05. ***
p
≤
.001.
3
4
5
6
7
Low
High
Quality of Social Features
Forecasted Happiness
Same-last
Vary-last & Control
Figure 1
Relationship between social features composite and fore-
casted happiness by experimental condition.
the easiest to predict and the most important of the
social features, and location was rated as both the easiest
to predict and the most important of the physical fea-
tures. By using these flagship variables as proxies for
social and physical qualities, we were able to include 121
out of 144 participants (84%) in our analysis.
This analysis revealed a very strong relationship
between anticipated location quality and forecasted hap-
piness,
t
(1,163) = 21.37,
B
= 0.52,
p
< .001, as well as a
smaller relationship between anticipated roommate
quality and expected happiness,
t
(1,163) = 5.29,
B
= 0.32,
p
< .001 (see Table 4). Consistent with the analysis using
composite variables, we found a significant Roommate
Quality
×
Condition interaction,
t
(1,163) = 2.27,
B
= 0.11,
p
< .05; participants in the same-last condition placed
greater weight on anticipated roommate quality than
did participants in the control and vary-last conditions
(see Figure 2). We also found a significant three-way
Location Quality
×
Roommate Quality
×
Condition
interaction,
t
(1,163)= 2.45,
B
= 0.04,
p
< .05.
6
Plotting this
interaction revealed that participants in the same-last
condition consistentlyused roommate quality in predict-
ing their future happiness, whereas the other partici-
pants placed weight on roommate quality only when
location was relatively good.
Although our first model combined all the relevant
variables and included 43% of the sample and our sec-
ond model used only the flagship variables and included
84% of the sample, results across the two analyses were
consistent. In both models, we found that participants
placed greater weight on physical features than social
features in forecasting their future happiness. More
important, we found that participants increased their
use of social features if they were asked to consider
aspects that would be similar across houses immediately
before forecasting. This provides support for our argu-
ment that participants gave low weight to social features
in making affective forecasts because of the isolation
effect. Our analyses, of course, rest on the assumption
that participants viewed physical features as more vari-
able than social features. This assumption was strongly
supported; participants reported thatthe quality of phys-
ical features would vary much more across houses (
M
=
5.33,
SD
= 0.74) than would the quality of social features
(
M
= 3.05,
SD
= 0.97),
t
(116) = 22.61,
p
< .0001.
By manipulating people’s focus on variables that
would vary little across houses, we found support for the
isolation effect in affective forecasting. It is possible, of
course, that other factors also contributed to people’s
tendency to underweight social features. Because of the
drive for certainty, forecasters may place excessive weight
on features that can be predicted with confidence. Par-
ticipants reported that the quality of physical features
was easier to predict (
M
= 5.28,
SD
= 0.87) than the qual-
ity of social features (
M
= 3.47,
SD
= 1.35),
t
(138) = 13.30,
p
< .0001, a difference that may have increased forecast-
ers’ tendency to attend to physical features over social
features.
Features related to actual happiness
. If social features
were completely unpredictable, participants would be
entirely justified in ignoring these features when making
forecasts. Although plausible, this interpretation is not
supported by the data; participants’ ratings of the actual
social quality of their assigned house at Time 2 corre-
lated strongly with their predicted ratings of social qual-
ity for that house at Time 1,
r
(21) = .60,
p
< .004, and this
correlation was similar in magnitude to the correlation
between predicted and actual physical quality ratings,
r
(32) = .52,
p
< .003.
1428
PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
TABLE 4:
Multilevel Model Predicting Forecasted Happiness From
Condition, Anticipated Roommate Quality, and Antici-
pated Location Quality
B
df
t
Fixed effects
Location quality
.52
1163
21.37***
Roommate quality
.32
1163
5.29***
Condition
–.05
119
–0.93
Roommate Quality
×
Condition
.11
1163
2.27*
Location Quality
×
Condition
–.01
1163
–0.49
Roommate Quality
×
Location
Quality
.05
1163
2.46*
Location Quality
×
Roommate
Quality
×
Condition
.04
1163
2.45*
Random effects
Intercept (individual differences
in forecasts)
.28
4.02***
Location
.03
3.32***
Roommate
.10
2.24*
Residual
1.04
22.54***
NOTE: Total observations = 1,728; observations used= 1,290;
R
2
= .55.
*
p
≤
.10. **
p
≤
.05. ***
p
≤
.001.
3
4
5
6
7
Low
High
Quality of Roommate Relationship
Forecasted Happiness
Same-last
Vary-last & Control
Figure 2
Relationship between roommate quality and forecasted
happiness by experimental condition.
More important, overall happiness at Time 2 was
better predicted by anticipated quality of social features
than by anticipated quality of physical features at Time 1.
We entered Time 1 ratings of anticipated social quality
and physical quality into a regression predicting overall
happiness at Time 2 and then entered the two-way inter-
action into a second model. Anticipated social quality at
Time 1 was marginally related to overall happiness at
Time 2,
t
(15) = 1.78,
β
= .65,
p
< . 10, whereas predicted
physicalqualitywasunrelatedtooverallhappiness,
t
(15)=
–0.17,
β
= –.06,
p
= .87. The two-way interaction was
nonsignificant,
t
< 1.
Unfortunately, these analyses included only 18 of the
90 participants who completed both surveys, due pri-
marily to missing data for the Time 1 social and physical
composite variables. Thus, we conducted an additional
analysis in which we substituted anticipated roommate
quality for the social quality composite and anticipated
quality of location for the physical quality composite,
which allowed us to include 65 out of 90 participants
(72%) in our analyses. We found that anticipated room-
mate quality significantly predicted actual happiness,
t
(62) = 3.12,
β
= .37,
p
< .003, whereas anticipated loca-
tion quality did not significantly predict actual happi-
ness,
t
(62) = 1.2,
β
= .14,
p
= .24. The two-way interaction
was not significant,
t
< 1.
Thus, anticipated social quality was at least as reliable
a predictor of future overall happiness as anticipated
physical quality. Another indication that participants in
Experiment 2 made valid predictions of social feature
quality is that these ratings were correlated with Experi-
ment 1 participants’ ratings of the actual quality of tutors
and sense of community in the houses,
r
(11) = .50,
p
<
.12; in other words, 1st-years’ perceptions of the social
quality of the houses corresponded with sophomores’
experiences in the houses. These findings cast doubt on
the interpretation that forecasters simply ignored social
features because their quality wasimpossible to predict.
Furthermore, it is important to note that our analyses
examining the relationship between forecasted happi-
ness and forecasted feature quality relied on those par-
ticipants who felt that they could predict the quality of
house features. The large number of “don’t know”
responses we received suggests that our survey enabled
participants to admit ignorance when appropriate. We
find it compelling that even participants who felt that
they could make reasonable predictions about the qual-
ity of social features ignored this information in making
affective forecasts. Still, the ease of predicting physical
feature quality relative to social feature quality may have
contributed to participants’ tendency to overweight the
latter and underweight the former. Future research may
confirm the role of subjective predictability in moderat-
ing the extent to which various features influence fore-
casts, but the present research provides the strongest evi-
dence for the role of perceived variability in moderating
the relative influence of various features; participants
placed excessive weight on highly variable physical fea-
tures relative to less variable social features, and this ten-
dency was attenuated when participants were led to
think about features that would be the same across
houses immediately before forecasting.
One might argue that ratings of social features were
unrelated to forecasted happiness because of a statistical
artifact: A relationship between social ratings and fore-
casted happiness could not emerge if there was insuffi-
cient variability in ratings of social features. Indeed,
there was greater variance in physical features (mean
SD
= 5.56) than social features (mean
SD
= 4.29) across indi-
viduals, within houses. Similarly, although relevant only
to the multilevel analyses, there was greater variance in
physical features (mean
SD
= 4.4) than social features
(mean
SD
= 1.76) across houses, within individuals. The
critical point, however, is that Time 1 ratings of social fea-
tures predicted overall happiness at Time 2 while failing
to predict Time 1 affective forecasts. In addition, the fact
that our manipulation of the isolation effect successfully
altered participants’ use of social features is inconsistent
with the possibility that ratings of social features were too
invariant to allow correlations with forecasts. Therefore,
it seems unlikely that Time 1 ratings of social features
failed to predict Time 1 affective forecasts primarily
because of insufficient variability.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
In two experiments, we found strong support for the
impact bias; participants predicted that housing assign-
ment would have far more influence on their emotional
well-being a year later than it actually did. Because we
used a longitudinal design and participants were ran-
domly assigned to a major life outcome that continued
to shape their lives at college, this study provides the
most powerful evidence to date for the impact bias. As
sophomores and juniors, participants’ actual happiness
was more consistently associated with their ratings of
social features of house life than with their ratings of
physical features of their house. Although these latter
results were correlational, our findings dovetail with pre-
vious research demonstrating that quality of social life is
moreimportanttohappinessthanarematerialcomforts.
When asked explicitly about the determinants of their
future happiness, participants recognized that social
aspects of house life would be more important thanphys-
ical qualities. Yet, Experiment 1 provided initial evi-
dence that participants expected to be much happier in
houses with good physical qualities, whereas we found
no evidence that participants based their forecasts on
the quality of social life in the houses. Indeed, partici-
Dunn et al. / MISPREDICTING SATISFACTION
1429
Your preview ends here
Eager to read complete document? Join bartleby learn and gain access to the full version
- Access to all documents
- Unlimited textbook solutions
- 24/7 expert homework help
pants who found themselves in houses with good physi-
cal features reported lower happiness than they had
anticipated.
Consistent with Study 1, participants in Study 2 placed
relatively little weight on social features in predicting
their future happiness while placing a great deal of
weight on physical features of the houses—despite the
fact that actual happiness a year later was related to the
perceived quality of social features at Time 1. Again, par-
ticipants recognized the important role social life would
play, reporting that social features mattered more on
average than did physical features when questioned
directly about determinants of happiness. Asking partici-
pants to consider what would be similar across houses
immediately before forecasting led them to place greater
weight on social features, which provides evidence that
the isolation effect plays a role in affective forecasting;
social features were generally disregarded because par-
ticipants paid little attention to important features that
were relatively constant across outcomes, unless explic-
itly instructed to do so.
Our results are interesting in light of the conflict
between research showing that people recognize that
social relationships are more important than material
comforts in producing happiness and other research
demonstrating that people’s behavior often flies in the
face of this stated belief (e.g., Putnam, 2000; Schor,
1991). The present research suggests that people may
truly believe that social relationships are more impor-
tant than material comforts, but they may lose sight of
this belief when imagining their happiness with alterna-
tive outcomes that vary more on material than social
dimensions. Again, we do not wish to argue that people
always underweight social features and overweight physi-
cal features. Exactly the opposite pattern might emerge
if participants were asked to predict their happiness
given a set of options that varied greatly on social dimen-
sions relative to physical dimensions (e.g., if they were
asked to imagine how happy they would be in the same
house with different sets of roommates). The key finding
of our research is that individuals may easily lose sight of
factors that they realize are important to their happiness
if these factors do not vary greatly across the options they
are imagining.
Of course, one might argue that the isolation effect
reflects a reasonable strategy rather than a dysfunctional
bias; perhaps it is wise to focus primarily on features that
differ widely between competing options while ignoring
features that are similar across options. We would argue
that this tendency might sometimes be functional when
choosing between options but makes far less sense when
imagining one’s future happiness given a specific out-
come. For example, it would be quite reasonable to
focus on location when choosing between houses that
varied a great deal on this dimension but not on others.
When imagining life in a specific house with a terrible
location, however, it would be unwise to expect lasting
misery while neglecting the happiness derived from liv-
ing with one’s wonderful roommates.
Limitations
. Because several of our findings depended
on correlational analyses, caution must be exercised in
interpreting our results. For example, the positive rela-
tionship between happiness and quality of social fea-
tures at Time 2 and Time 3 could indicate that both of
these variables depended on a third variable, that happi-
ness leads to greater satisfaction with social relation-
ships, or that strong social relationships promote happi-
ness. Research by Oishi and Diener (2001) suggests that
general happiness is more likely to influence global rat-
ings of social relationships than to influence more spe-
cific ratings used in our study (e.g., relationships with
roommates). In any case, based on previous research, it
seems reasonable to assume that the relationship
between social life in the house and happiness is proba-
bly bidirectional, meaning that high-quality social rela-
tionships have at least some positive influence on
happiness.
It is important to remember that not all of our analy-
ses were correlational; participants were randomly
assigned to houses and we randomly assigned people to
condition in Study 2. If this were not the case, one might
argue that good physical features failed to promote hap-
piness in a consistent fashion because people who seek
out material comforts are more difficult to please than
their less-materialistic peers. Yet, in our research, partici-
pants were assigned at random to houses that would be
expensive on the open market (because of their central
locations, beautiful architecture, and large rooms) or to
houses that would be relatively inexpensive. Thus,
although the form of our analyses precludes strong
causal inferences, the internal validity of the present
research is stronger than most studies on the correlates
of well-being.
Future directions
. Because our research tested these
ideas in the single domain of a university housing system,
it would be worthwhile to expand this line of research
through laboratory studies or field studies in a different
domain. Although researchers have already examined
the spurious factors that influence reports of subjective
well-being (e.g., Schwarz & Clore, 1983), we believe that
special biases come into play at the forecasting stage. As
well as focusing exclusively on features that vary between
outcomes, affective forecasters may place disproportion-
ate weight on features that are easy to predict or visual-
ize. Future research should focus on examining these
variables directly to test whether forecasters show a
1430
PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
systematic bias toward placing excessive weight on easily
predictable and visualizable features.
Policy implications
. Our research suggests that individ-
uals may sometimes err by placing excessive weight on
highly variable features and inadequate weight on less
variable features in imagining the emotional conse-
quences of competing options. A similar bias may
emerge when policy makers envision options meant to
increase the well-being of their constituents. For exam-
ple, in the “slum clearance” projects of the 1950s and
1960s, urban planners chose to tear down small, dilapi-
dated tenement buildings, constructing massive towers
in their place. Policy makers apparently focused on the
drastic improvement in physical features that this
change would provide and overlooked the ramifications
for social life of ripping up small, tightly knit tenement
communities to create giant, anonymous collections of
tower dwellers (Ross & Nisbett, 1991). Thus, individuals
and policy makers alike may go astray if, as in our studies,
they focus on alternatives that vary a great deal on fea-
tures (e.g., physical aspects of tenements vs. high-rise
apartments) that will be relatively inconsequential for
people’s well-being.
NOTES
1. After asking participants to report their overall happiness, we
asked them to report their happiness with their house, specifically.
When we substituted house-specific happiness for overall happiness in
the analyses reported in this article, highly consistent results emerged.
Therefore, only results for overall happiness are reported, but results
of the house-specific analyses are available from the authors.
2. We used this idiographic approach to assess whether a given
house assignment represented a positive or negative outcome for each
individual. This approach was useful because evaluations of the houses
varied from individual to individual, although there was substantial
agreementabout the relativedesirabilityof the houses; the averagecor-
relation between any one participant’s forecasted happiness and the
overall sample’s forecasted happiness for the 12 houses was
r
= .69,
p
<
.0001.
3. Although the negative relationship between quality of physical
features and happiness was unexpected, other studies have found a
negative relationship between increases in material comfort (e.g.,
money) and well-being (e.g., Thoits & Hannan, 1979). Although this
paradoxical finding is intriguing, it was inconsistent across time points
and should be interpreted with caution.
4. Many participants failed to provide complete data for all 12
houses, resulting in a large amount of missing data in each of the 12
regressions. This potential problem is dealt with in detail in the subse-
quent multilevel modeling section.
5. Although not of central interest in this article, the model also
provides estimates of the random effects. The intercept estimate indi-
cates that thereweresignificant individualdifferencesin meanlevelsof
forecasted happiness, collapsing across houses; in other words, some
people expected to be happier than others. The small random effects
of physical and social quality indicate that the weight placed on these
variables varied little across participants.
6. The only other significant effect was an unpredicted two-way
Location Quality
×
Roommate Quality interaction. Because this inter-
action was not consistent across models and did not involve condition,
it will not be discussed further.
REFERENCES
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991).
Multiple regression: Testing and inter-
preting interactions
. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Argyle, M. (1999). Causes and correlates of happiness. In D. Kahneman,
E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.),
Well-being: The foundations of
hedonic psychology
(pp. 353-373). New York: Russell Sage.
Biswah-Diener, R., & Diener, E. (2001). Making the best of a bad situa-
tion: Satisfaction in the slums of Calcutta.
Social Indicators Research
,
55
, 329-352.
Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992).
Hierarchical linear models:
Applications and data analysis methods
. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Buehler, R., & McFarland, C. (2001). Intensity bias in affective fore-
casting: The role of temporal focus.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,
27
, 1480-1493.
Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rodgers, W. L. (1976).
The quality of
American life: Perceptions, evaluations, and satisfactions
. New York:
Russell Sage.
Diener, E., Gohm, C. L., Suh, E., & Oishi, S. (2000). Similarity of the
relations between marital status and subjective well-being across
cultures.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology
,
31
, 419-436.
Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Very happy people.
Psychologi-
cal Science
,
13
, 81-84.
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective
well-being: Three decades of progress.
Psychological Bulletin
,
125
,
276-302.
Fordyce, M. W. (1988). A review of research on the happiness mea-
sures: A sixty second index of happiness and mental health.
Social
Indicators Research
,
20
, 355-381.
Freedman, J. (1978).
Happy people
. New York: Harcourt Brace
Jovanovich.
Gilbert, D. T., Driver-Linn, E., & Wilson, T. D. (2002). The trouble
with Vronsky: Impact bias in the forecasting of future affective
states. In P. Salovey & L. Feldman-Barrett (Eds.),
The wisdom of feel-
ing
(pp. 144-166). New York: Guilford.
Gilbert, D. T., Lieberman, M. D., & Wilson, T. D. (in press).
The pecu-
liar longevity of things not so bad
. Unpublished manuscript, Harvard
University.
Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley,
T. P. (1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affec-
tive forecasting.
Journal of Personality & Social Psychology
,
75
, 617-
638.
Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2000). Miswanting: Some problems in
the forecasting of future affective states. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.),
Feel-
ing and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition. Studies in emotion
and social interaction, second series
(pp. 178-197). New York: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Hodges, S. D. (1997). When matching up features messes up deci-
sions: The role of feature matching in successive choices.
Journal of
Personality & Social Psychology
,
72
, 1310-1321.
Houston, D. A., & Sherman, S. J. (1995). Cancellation and focus: The
role of shared and unique features in the choice process.
Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology
,
31
, 357-378.
Houston, D. A., Sherman, S. J., & Baker, S. M. (1991). Feature match-
ing, unique features, and the dynamics of the choice process:
Predecision conflict and postdecision satisfaction.
Journal of Exper-
imental Social Psychology
,
27
, 411-430.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of
decision under risk.
Econometrica
,
47
, 263-291.
Kashy, D. A., & Kenny, D. A. (2000). The analysis of data from dyads
and groups. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.),
Handbook of research
methods in social and personality psychology
(pp. 451-477). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Data analysis in social
psychology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.),
Hand-
book of social psychology
(Vol. 1, pp. 233-265). New York: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Larson, R. W. (1990). The solitary side of life: An examination of the
time people spend alone from childhood to old age.
Developmental
Review
,
10
, 155-183.
Dunn et al. / MISPREDICTING SATISFACTION
1431
Loewenstein, G. (1996, May 3).
Explaining public indifference to income
inequality
. Paper presented to the MacArthur Foundation Meeting
on Inequality, Cambridge, MA.
Loewenstein, G., & Schkade, D. (1999). Wouldn’t it be nice? Pre-
dicting future feelings. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz
(Eds.),
Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology
(pp. 85-105).
New York: Russell Sage.
Mitchell, T. R., Thompson, L., Peterson, E., & Cronk, R. (1997). Tem-
poral adjustments in the evaluation of events: The “rosy view.”
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
,
33
, 421-448.
Myers, D. G. (1999). Close relationships and quality of life. In D.
Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.),
Well-being: The founda-
tions of hedonic psychology
(pp. 374-391). New York: Russell Sage.
Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2001). Re-examining the general positivity
model of subjective well-being: The discrepancy between specific
and global domain satisfaction.
Journal of Personality
,
69
, 641-666.
Pettijohn, T. F., II., & Pettijohn, T. F. (1996). Perceived happiness of
college students measured by Maslow’s hierarchy of needs.
Psycho-
logical Reports
,
79
, 759-762.
Putnam, R. D. (2000).
Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American
community
. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991).
The person and the situation: Perspectives
of social psychology
. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Schkade, D. A., & Kahneman, D. (1998). Does living in California
make people happy? A focusing illusion in judgments of life satis-
faction.
Psychological Science
,
9
, 340-346.
Schor, J. (1991).
The overworked American: The unexpected decline of lei-
sure
. New York: Basic Books.
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judg-
ments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affec-
tive states.
Journal of Personality & Social Psychology
,
45
, 513-523.
Sheldon, K. M., Elliot, A. J., Kim, Y., & Kasser, T. (2001). What is satisfy-
ing about satisfying events? Testing 10 candidate psychological
needs.
Journal of Personality & Social Psychology
,
80
, 325-339.
Sieff, E. M., Dawes, R. M., & Loewenstein, G. (1999). Anticipated ver-
sus actual reaction to HIV test results.
American Journal of Psychol-
ogy
,
112
, 297-311.
Thoits, P., & Hannan, M. (1979). Income and psychological distress:
The impact of an income-maintenance experiment.
Journal of
Health & Social Behavior
,
20
, 120-138.
Tversky, A. (1972). Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice.
Psycho-
logical Review
,
79
, 281-299.
Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (in press). Affective forecasting. In M. P.
Zanna (Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology
. San Diego,
CA: Academic Press.
Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T., Meyers, J. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Axsom, D.
(2000). Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecast-
ing.
Journal of Personality & Social Psychology
,
78
, 821-836.
Received June 11, 2002
Revision accepted December 26, 2002
1432
PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
Your preview ends here
Eager to read complete document? Join bartleby learn and gain access to the full version
- Access to all documents
- Unlimited textbook solutions
- 24/7 expert homework help
Related Documents
Recommended textbooks for you

Ciccarelli: Psychology_5 (5th Edition)
Psychology
ISBN:9780134477961
Author:Saundra K. Ciccarelli, J. Noland White
Publisher:PEARSON

Cognitive Psychology
Psychology
ISBN:9781337408271
Author:Goldstein, E. Bruce.
Publisher:Cengage Learning,

Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and ...
Psychology
ISBN:9781337565691
Author:Dennis Coon, John O. Mitterer, Tanya S. Martini
Publisher:Cengage Learning

Psychology in Your Life (Second Edition)
Psychology
ISBN:9780393265156
Author:Sarah Grison, Michael Gazzaniga
Publisher:W. W. Norton & Company

Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research a...
Psychology
ISBN:9781285763880
Author:E. Bruce Goldstein
Publisher:Cengage Learning

Theories of Personality (MindTap Course List)
Psychology
ISBN:9781305652958
Author:Duane P. Schultz, Sydney Ellen Schultz
Publisher:Cengage Learning
Recommended textbooks for you
- Ciccarelli: Psychology_5 (5th Edition)PsychologyISBN:9780134477961Author:Saundra K. Ciccarelli, J. Noland WhitePublisher:PEARSONCognitive PsychologyPsychologyISBN:9781337408271Author:Goldstein, E. Bruce.Publisher:Cengage Learning,Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and ...PsychologyISBN:9781337565691Author:Dennis Coon, John O. Mitterer, Tanya S. MartiniPublisher:Cengage Learning
- Psychology in Your Life (Second Edition)PsychologyISBN:9780393265156Author:Sarah Grison, Michael GazzanigaPublisher:W. W. Norton & CompanyCognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research a...PsychologyISBN:9781285763880Author:E. Bruce GoldsteinPublisher:Cengage LearningTheories of Personality (MindTap Course List)PsychologyISBN:9781305652958Author:Duane P. Schultz, Sydney Ellen SchultzPublisher:Cengage Learning

Ciccarelli: Psychology_5 (5th Edition)
Psychology
ISBN:9780134477961
Author:Saundra K. Ciccarelli, J. Noland White
Publisher:PEARSON

Cognitive Psychology
Psychology
ISBN:9781337408271
Author:Goldstein, E. Bruce.
Publisher:Cengage Learning,

Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and ...
Psychology
ISBN:9781337565691
Author:Dennis Coon, John O. Mitterer, Tanya S. Martini
Publisher:Cengage Learning

Psychology in Your Life (Second Edition)
Psychology
ISBN:9780393265156
Author:Sarah Grison, Michael Gazzaniga
Publisher:W. W. Norton & Company

Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research a...
Psychology
ISBN:9781285763880
Author:E. Bruce Goldstein
Publisher:Cengage Learning

Theories of Personality (MindTap Course List)
Psychology
ISBN:9781305652958
Author:Duane P. Schultz, Sydney Ellen Schultz
Publisher:Cengage Learning
Browse Popular Homework Q&A
Q: Alice wants to estimate the percentage of people who plan on voting yes for the upcoming school…
Q: The following protein sequence was made.
N-MET TYR GLN GLN CYS-C
a. Using the top most option from…
Q: 3.8
4.0
4.2
4.4
g' (z) -0.8
2.2 1.8
-1.2
Selected values of the derivative of the function g are…
Q: Discuss the notion of Manifest Destiny.
Is the idea rooted in a firm belief in a Christian God, or…
Q: An orca with a mass of 70,000kg is floating partially submerged in the Pacific Ocean. What is the…
Q: Stryker Industries received an offer from an exporter for 29,000 units of product at $19 per unit.…
Q: 3. Frank's utility is given by U = InX+2Y, where Mux = 10/x and MU 2. Prices are Px = 2, Px=4 and M…
Q: A cannon ball is fired with an initial velocity vo at a wall that is 100. meters tall and located…
Q: Given the value of one trigonometric function of an acute angle 0, find the values of the remaining…
Q: What is the pathway of a protein used for detoxification in the liver with a galactose modification.…
Q: -cy
-4
1. How are the two compounds below related? R
CH3
= 4-5-cn₂
cug
си,
Ck₂
+
CUS
CH₂
K
f
H.
H…
Q: Consider the following algorithm:
sum = 0
For j starting at 1 and ending with 11:
sum =…
Q: A tube is being stretched while maintaining its cylindrical shape. The height is increasing at the…
Q: Two-years ago, Trans-Atlantic Airlines sold a $250 million bond issue to finance the purchase of new…
Q: Compute Lead Time
Jackson Fabricators Inc. machines metal parts for the
automotive industry. Under…
Q: A college is considering investing $6 million to add 10,000 seats to its football stadium. The…
Q: A pseudo-random signal (S) of -3 or 3 is transmitted over a noisy channel. S=-3 is transmitted with…
Q: For this problem you must use Java’s semaphore class. Your implementation for this
problem must only…
Q: - 3
Let u =
7
and v = 6
Compute and compare u •v, ||u||2, v|2, and ||u + v||2. Do not use the…
Q: 7. Let f(x) = 4x − 3, g(x) = x² + 4, h(x) = x + 5. Evaluate the following.
a. (f+g)(-4)
Q: Why should you use StringBuffer objects instead of String objects in a
program that makes lot of…
Q: Velocity
O
3
Time, s
(1)
st
Velocity
Velocity +
Time
(4)
Time, s
(2)
Velocity +
Velocity +
Time, s…
Q: A certain index I is a measure of soil moisture. An article described the rate of change of I with…
Q: Explain two ways African American institutions and resources sup-
ported African American…
Q: Write an equation for an exponential decay function where the initial
population is 2700 and the…