pdf
keyboard_arrow_up
School
Arizona State University *
*We aren’t endorsed by this school
Course
380
Subject
Psychology
Date
Oct 30, 2023
Type
Pages
11
Uploaded by bluesaturn110
ORIGINAL RESEARCH
published: 29 March 2018
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00429
Edited by:
Antonios Dakanalis,
Università degli Studi di
Milano-Bicocca, Italy
Reviewed by:
Andrea Wyssen,
University of Fribourg, Switzerland
Samantha Jane Brooks,
University of Cape Town, South Africa
*Correspondence:
Bernadetta Izydorczyk
b.izydorczyk@interia.pl
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Eating Behavior,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
Received:
17 November 2017
Accepted:
14 March 2018
Published:
29 March 2018
Citation:
Izydorczyk B and
Sitnik-Warchulska K (2018)
Sociocultural Appearance Standards
and Risk Factors for Eating Disorders
in Adolescents and Women of Various
Ages. Front. Psychol. 9:429.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00429
Sociocultural Appearance Standards
and Risk Factors for Eating
Disorders in Adolescents and
Women of Various Ages
Bernadetta Izydorczyk
*
and Katarzyna Sitnik-Warchulska
Institute of Applied Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland
The main aim of the present study was to verify the level of impact of sociocultural
appearance standards (passive awareness and active internalization) have on body
dissatisfaction, the desire to engage in a relentless pursuit of thinness, the adoption
of a perfectionistic attitude toward the body, and the development of a tendency to
engage in bulimic eating behavior, which can develop in adolescent girls and women
of varying ages. The study group comprised 234 individuals: 95 secondary school girls,
33 high school girls, 56 female students, and 50 employed women, all of whom were
living in southern Poland. Participants were not diagnosed with any psychiatric disorders
(including eating disorders). The variables were measured using the Polish version
of Garner’s Eating Disorder Inventory and the Polish Sociocultural Attitudes Towards
Physical Appearance and Body Image Inventory [based on the SATAQ-3 (Sociocultural
Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire Scale-3)]. The findings revealed that the
youngest Polish girls (aged 12–15) reported the highest level of risk factors for eating
disorders. Among the entire study group, the internalization of appearance standards
and the pressure associated with various media messages were determined to be
predictors of the pursuit of thinness, regardless of age and body mass index values.
The second most significant variable explained by the internalization of sociocultural
standards was body dissatisfaction. The internalization of sociocultural norms provided
a significant explanation of bulimic tendencies only in the youngest girls. Perfectionism
proved not to be affected by the sociocultural impact of mass media. The adult
women had the lowest average scores over the entire study population regarding
exposure frequency to body images in mass media and regarding the experience of
pressure exerted by sociocultural norms. The high level of internalization of sociocultural
appearance standards seems to be significantly linked to body satisfaction in women
aged 30 and older. Young adolescent girls constitute a high-risk group for a specific
psychological proneness to developing eating disorders as a result of the sociocultural
influence exerted by mass media. The obtained study results can prove helpful in
creating education programs in preventive healthcare aimed particularly at the youngest
adolescents.
Keywords: body-image, eating disorders, mass media, girls, women
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
1
March 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 429
Izydorczyk and Sitnik-Warchulska
Sociocultural Appearance Standards in Women
INTRODUCTION
Previous literature reviews have confirmed the importance of
sociocultural messages in highly industrialized countries in
regard to strengthening the perception that “thin female bodies”
are attractive (
Levine et al., 1994
;
Bearman et al., 2006
;
Benton
and Karazsia, 2015
). Such sociocultural standards concerning
body image and evaluations of attractiveness are commonly
propagated in mass media. In particular, the need for obtaining
social approval concerning how one looks and whether one’s
body is considered “attractive” is important for adolescent
girls and young women in terms of their self-esteem (
Clark
and Tiggemann, 2008
;
Izydorczyk and Rybicka-Klimczyk, 2009
;
McCabe and Ricciardelli, 2009
;
Cash, 2011
;
Ferguson K. et al.,
2011
;
Finne et al., 2011
;
Mond et al., 2011
;
Gajtkowska, 2013
;
Józefik, 2014
;
Izydorczyk, 2015a
;
Czepczor et al., 2016
;
Glogowska
and Zatorska, 2016
).
Many studies have confirmed that women more strongly
and regularly feel dissatisfaction with their appearance and,
as a result, many intensely pursue the sociocultural ideal of
the female body, based on slenderness and thinness, that is
promoted in the media (
Grabe et al., 2008
;
Cash, 2011
;
Ferguson
C.J. et al., 2011
); it has been shown that such an unrealistic
ideal of a slim figure can result in body dissatisfaction and
disordered eating (
Rodgers et al., 2011
). In fact, studies have
found that there is a positive correlation between exposure to
mass media (TV and magazines) and body dissatisfaction, thin-
ideal internalization, and disordered eating (
Thomsen et al., 2002
;
Levine and Murnen, 2009
). The influence of the media has a
key impact on appearance satisfaction in terms of thinness and
fatness (
Levine and Murnen, 2009
).
Levine and Smolak (2006)
indicated that women’s dissatisfaction with their bodies is a
derivative of three components: the idealization of slenderness
and leanness, an irrational fear of fat, and a conviction that weight
and shape are central determinants of one’s identity.
Factors such as body-image dissatisfaction, the restrictive
pursuit of thinness, the adoption of a perfectionistic attitude
toward the body, and the development of bulimic tendencies
are often indicated in scientific research as predictors of eating
disorders (
Stice et al., 1994, 2013
;
Tiggemann, 2003
;
Garner, 2004
;
Jones and Crawford, 2006
;
˙
Zechowski, 2008
;
Striegel-Moore
et al., 2009
;
Izydorczyk, 2015b
). However, most researchers focus
on selected risk factor. The clinical experience of the authors
indicates that the risk factors of eating disorders should not
be considered separately. These factors seem to constitute the
specific syndrome, related to the culture of thinness.
It would be a great simplification to say that mass media
has a direct impact on the occurrence of eating disorders, and
research results are ambiguous in this respect (
Stice et al., 1994,
1998
;
Tiggemann, 2003
).
Rodgers (2016)
proposed a theoretical
model that suggests the existence of two possible mechanisms
behind the relationship between promoted appearance standards
and
behaviors
that
are
considered
risk
factors
for
eating
disorders. The first mechanism is based on the assumption
that sociocultural discourse on healthy weight promotes the
internalization of anti-fat attitudes (in regard to oneself and
toward others) and the need to control weight. The second
mechanism concerns the formation of the belief that lifestyle
factors (such as diet or physical activity) play an important
role in weight control. As a consequence, this can lead to a
preoccupation with weight, which is then followed by disordered
eating and excessive exercise.
Models of media influence argue that normative beliefs
concerning society, such as those shown in media, become
attitudes about the self (
Levine and Smolak, 1996
;
Levine and
Murnen, 2009
). For example,
Levine and Murnen (2009)
, in their
critical review, indicated that mass media should be treated as a
variable risk factor that might later become a causal risk factor.
In their meta-analysis, they indicate that when considering the
relationship between the media and eating disorders, variables
such as awareness of the importance in society of a thin ideal,
the internalization of this ideal, and perceived pressure from the
media to be thin should be examined.
It is also worth highlighting that contemporary research has
applied a large variety of methods to measure the variables
presented above. One valid measure for these variables is
the
Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire
(SATAQ;
Knauss et al., 2009
;
Sánchez-Carracedo et al., 2012
).
However, many researchers use this scale mainly to study
the relationship between sociocultural appearance standards,
promoted in the media, and selected risk factors concerning
eating disorders (e.g., body dissatisfaction;
Stice and Whitenton,
2002
). Definitions of these factors also differ; some researchers
use the
Eating Disorders Inventory
(EDI) in their research, but
this is not a standard. Moreover, there is a lack of research on the
relationships between sociocultural standards of appearance and
the entire team of risk factors involved in eating disorders.
Efforts to identify sociocultural predictors that contribute to
the development of body-image distortion as a result of eating
disorders are not very common in studies conducted on adult
women (aged 30 and older) when compared to studies on
adolescents. Further, as mentioned above, there is also a lack of
studies focusing on broad populations of women (young and
adult) with the goal of measuring the differences between them
and the strength of the correlations between factors commonly
considered risk factors for eating disorders.
There is also doubt concerning whether the sociocultural
standards of appearance are universal. The culture of thinness
is common in Western Europe, the United States, and more
often on the other continents.
Nielson et al. (2013)
indicate that
exposure to Western models in the media has likely contributed
to a rejection of stereotypical Asian facial features. The European
findings show that young people from the United Kingdom seem
to demonstrate the most intense internalization of the thin ideal
and pressure from the media to look a certain way (
Argyrides,
2013
).
Swami et al. (2010)
found that the heavier bodies may be
preferred in low-socioeconomic-status sites compared to high-
socioeconomic-status sites in Malaysia and South Africa, but not
in Austria.
However, the cross-cultural explorations suggest that overall
differences are limited and mainly associated with differences in
socioeconomic development (
Swami et al., 2010
). Considering
the above, it seems important to analyze girls and women of
different ages and from the same sociocultural area. Further, in
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
2
March 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 429
Izydorczyk and Sitnik-Warchulska
Sociocultural Appearance Standards in Women
light of the findings of numerous studies concerning the impact of
sociocultural standards in Western, highly developed countries, it
appears that conducting such research in dynamically developing
countries in Central and Eastern Europe would be valuable.
Moreover, this type of research can also support meta-analyses,
taking into account different populations.
The authors’ research for this article comprised a project that
was performed over several years, which concerned searching
for sociocultural risk factors for eating disorders among groups
of Polish girls and women. Poland is a dynamically developed
country.
In
the
conducted
research,
the
authors
decided
to use SATAQ Scale-3 (SATAQ-3) to measure sociocultural
appearance standards and EDI to measure variables recognized
in the literature as risk factors for eating disorders (such as
dissatisfaction with the body, the restrictive pursuit of slimness,
and bulimic tendencies). The feature of perfectionism (measured
by EDI) was also included in the scope of variables, because of its
importance for the development and course of eating disorders.
The authors assumed that mass media are a major part of the
lives of many children, adolescents, and adults. The assimilation
of sociocultural appearance standards, promoted in the media, is
of key importance. This idea is supported by research showing
that it is not just the frequent exposure to the media, but also
referring to media content to oneself that leads to an assessment
of one’s own body (
Wilcox and Laird, 2000
). Therefore, it can be
concluded that this process requires a degree of awareness and is
dependent on intrapsychic processes. For this reason the authors
decided to focus on the analysis of the self-perception, individual
attitude to media consumption and individual assessment of
media impact. These factors are little understood. The main
aim of the present study was to empirically verify the level of
impact sociocultural appearance standards (awareness and active
internalization) have on body dissatisfaction, the desire to engage
in a relentless pursuit of thinness, the adoption of a perfectionistic
attitude toward the body, and the development of a tendency
to engage in bulimic eating behavior (self-inducing vomiting
and using other purgative behaviors to eliminate food from the
stomach), which can develop in adolescent girls and women of
varying ages.
Specifically, the research objective was to seek answers to the
following research questions:
(1) Are sociocultural standards of appearance related to the
development of risk factors for eating disorders (dissatisfaction
with the body, restrictive pursuit of slimness, bulimic tendencies,
and perfectionism) in modern-day girls and women?
(2) Is the relationship between sociocultural standards of
appearance and risk factors of eating disorders specific to
the developmental period (early adolescence, middle and late
adolescence, young adulthood, middle adulthood)?
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Procedure
This study was conducted between 2012 and 2016 in southern
Poland.
The
authors
planned
to
examine
several
hundred
women, and it was decided to take into account developmental
differences over various stages of life (second research question).
For this reason, four age-based subgroups were distinguished:
12–15 (early adolescence), 16–20 (middle and late adolescence),
21–29
(young
adulthood),
and
women
over
30
(middle
adulthood). The applied divisions into subgroups corresponds
to
the
stages
of
development
indicated
in
developmental
psychology, and takes into account the age ranges of the girls and
women who were examined in related research.
The categorizations for the research subgroup were based
on a clinical interview [questions concerning age, previous and
required treatment of eating disorders, mental disorders, and
body mass index (BMI)]. Due to the purpose of the research
(examination of risk factors, not symptoms of eating disorders),
the study only included persons without mental disorders;
specifically, the exclusion criteria were: disability and related
visible deformities of the body; previous diagnosis of anorexia,
bulimia, or other types of eating disorders; and mental disorders
associated with the development of a distorted body image
(psychotic, dysmorphia).
Based on literary sources, research findings, and the authors’
clinical observations, the following two main variables were
defined in the study design:
(1) Risk factors for eating disorders: a criterion (dependent)
variable that comprises four elements:
-
Body dissatisfaction: a variable describing the severity of
a negative emotional attitude toward one’s own body, its
shape, and measurements.
-
Pursuit of thinness: a variable describing the severity of
preoccupation with the pursuit of thinness, the desire to
have a thinner body, and increased anxiety and concerns
regarding gaining weight.
-
Bulimic
tendencies:
a
variable
describing
a
specific
cognitive disposition related to a lack of control of eating
behavior that defines the severity of obsessive thoughts
concerning eating, binge eating, and provoking various
compensatory reactions of the body (self-induced vomiting,
using laxatives without any medical justification).
-
Perfectionism:
a
variable
describing
an
individual’s
tendency
to
form
excessively
high
expectations
of
himself/herself and seek to fulfill the highest possible
standards in terms of personal achievements and activities
in life, leading him/her to engage in excessive self-criticism
of his/her body, successes, and achievements.
(2)
Sociocultural
appearance
standards:
a
predictor
(independent) variable, defined by a person’s level of adaptation
to
sociocultural
standards
of
body
appearance
promoted
in the media (audio and visual media messages concerning
appearance). This variable comprises three elements:
-
Internalization: a variable describing the assimilation level
(strength) of norms concerning appearance created by
modern culture and mass media (TV, radio, magazines and
newspapers, advertisements, etc.).
-
Pressure: a variable describing the level (strength) of
the pressure experienced from various media messages
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
3
March 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 429
Your preview ends here
Eager to read complete document? Join bartleby learn and gain access to the full version
- Access to all documents
- Unlimited textbook solutions
- 24/7 expert homework help
Izydorczyk and Sitnik-Warchulska
Sociocultural Appearance Standards in Women
(TV, radio, magazines and newspapers, advertisements,
etc.) promoting body-image standards.
-
Exposure
to
body
images
promoted
in
mass
media:
a variable describing the frequency of consumption of
various information promoted in mass media pertaining to
body-image standards and norms of appearance.
An additional control variable used in the study was BMI,
which comprises a value derived from the body mass divided by
the square of the body height in meters (
Menzel et al., 2011
).
On this index, optimal weight is assumed to range from 19.5
to 24.5, with values below the average indicating underweight
or a pathological loss of weight, and values above indicating
overweight.
Participants
The study involved initially 250 girls and women, 16 of whom
were excluded, after the clinical interview. There were six girls
aged 12–15 treated for anorexia, five girls aged 16–20 treated
for anorexia and binge eating disorder, and five women over 30
treated for chronic depression.
The study group finally comprised 234 individuals: secondary
school girls, high school girls, female students (full-time and part-
time students attending courses in the fields of humanities, social
studies, and biological science), and employed women, all of
whom were living in southern Poland. The research group was
homogeneous in terms of sociodemographic conditions (place of
residence, age, gender). The following subgroups were created:
Subgroup I (95 adolescent girls aged 12–15; these represented
early adolescence; mean age = 14.0, mean BMI = 19.0)
Subgroup II (33 adolescent girls aged 16–20; these represented
middle
and
late
adolescence;
mean
age
=
18.2,
mean
BMI = 20.0)
Subgroup III (56 women aged 21–29; these represented young
adulthood; mean age = 25.5; mean BMI = 23.0)
Subgroup IV (50 women aged 30 and older; these represented
middle adulthood; mean age = 35.5, mean BMI = 24.4)
The criteria applied when selecting participants for these
subgroups included: participants’ ages (12–50 years), BMIs
(optimal BMI values at a given age), and the absence of any
eating disorders (e.g., anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa) that
required treatment. The BMI values of all the adolescent girls and
women participating in the study were within normal limits and
corresponded to the optimal weight (18.5–24.9).
Compliance With Ethical Standards
This
study
was
conducted
in
accordance
with
the
recommendations
of
an
institutional
research
committee
(Research
Ethics
Committee
of
the
Institute
of
Applied
Psychology,
Jagiellonian
University,
Krakow)
and
was
determined to conform with the 1964 Helsinki declaration
and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards,
as written informed consent was obtained from all subjects.
Informed consent was also obtained from the parents or legal
guardians for all participants under the age of 16.
The protocol was approved by the Research Ethics Committee
of the Institute of Applied Psychology, Jagiellonian University,
Krakow.
Instruments
The clinical interview was used to select study participants. The
interview questions concerned the objective indicators, such as
age, body weight, and height (to estimate the BMI), the presence
of disabilities and related visible deformities of the body, previous
diagnosis of eating disorders (anorexia, bulimia, or other types
of eating disorders), and mental disorders associated with the
development of distorted body image (body dysmorphic disorder,
psychotic disorders, personality disorders, depression, bipolar
disorders).
The predictor variable, and its components, were measured
using
the
Polish
Sociocultural
Attitudes
Towards
Physical
Appearance and Body Image Inventory (SATPABI), which was
developed by
Izydorczyk (2014)
. It is based on the SATAQ-
3 developed by
Thompson et al. (2004)
; the SATAQ-3 is
a measure of the internalization of appearance ideals and
appearance
pressures.
To
create
the
SATPABI,
a
24-item
Polish-language questionnaire was drafted based on a double
back-translation of the English version of the SATAQ-3. The
said questionnaire was used in a pilot study on a population
of 140 Polish females who did not manifest traits of eating
disorders. The items employed in the pilot study described
basic manifestations of assimilated sociocultural appearance
standards
regarding the
following
variables:
internalization,
pressure, and exposure to body images promoted in mass
media. The obtained pilot study results were then subjected
to
a
reliability
analysis,
in
which
sampling
adequacy
was
estimated
at
KMO
=
0.983.
Bartlett’s
test
results
allowed
the
authors
to
disprove
the
identity
matrix
hypothesis
(
χ
2
= 3046.280;
df
= 276;
p
<
000). Further, in order to
verify and estimate statistical indicators for all positions in
the questionnaire, a factor analysis was conducted and the
identified factors were subjected to varimax rotation with Kaiser
normalization.
The
SATPABI,
like
the
SATAQ-3,
has
three
subscales,
assessing internalization (general, athlete; nine items), pressure
(seven items), and exposure to body images promoted in mass
media (eight items); each item on each scale is given between one
and five points. For each scale, the higher the sum of points, the
greater the severity of the indicator.
The criterion variable and its components were measured
using the Polish version of Garner’s EDI, which was developed
by
˙
Zechowski (
Garner, 2004
;
˙
Zechowski, 2008
) (Psychological
Assessment Resources granted permission for one of the authors
to conduct research using the EDI). The total scores for the
dissatisfaction with the body, drive for thinness, bulimia, and
perfectionism scales in the EDI questionnaire were used as
indicators of risk factors for eating disorders. For each scale, the
higher the sum of the points (body dissatisfaction: nine items,
pursuit of thinness: seven items, bulimic tendencies: seven items,
and perfectionism: six items; each item of each scale is scored
between zero and three points), the greater the severity of the
indicator.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
4
March 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 429
Izydorczyk and Sitnik-Warchulska
Sociocultural Appearance Standards in Women
Statistical Methods
The examined subgroups differed in terms of the number of
members, but the size of each subgroup was sufficient to conduct
the planned statistical analyses.
The
first
stage
of
the
statistical
analysis
contained
measurements of mean values relating to the severity of each
variable (perfectionism, body dissatisfaction, leanness, bulimic
tendencies), and mean values (
M
) and standard deviations
(
SD
) concerning the intensification of explanatory variable
factors: internalization, pressure of sociocultural norms, and
searching for information about body image in mass media. The
significance was measured using one-way analysis of variance
(ANOVA) with the least significant difference (LSD) test applied
post hoc
.
Further,
a
stepwise
regression
analysis
was
applied
to
determine the extent to which sociocultural appearance standards
explain
the
development
of
excessive
body
dissatisfaction,
the pathological and relentless pursuit of thinness, bulimic
tendencies, and perfectionism in the studied subgroups of girls
and women.
RESULTS
Characteristics of the Dependent
Variable in the Participants
Figure 1
shows the statistically significant differences between
the various subgroups in terms of the average severity of
FIGURE 1 |
Statistics concerning the differences between the subgroups regarding the severity of risk of developing eating disorders.
TABLE 1 |
Descriptive statistics of the criterion and predictor variables (
n
= 234).
Age
(group)
Perfectionism
Pursuit of
thinness
Bulimic
tendencies
Body
dissatisfaction
Internalization of
sociocultural
norms
Exposure to
body image in
mass media
Pressure of
sociocultural
norms
M
(
SD
)
M
(
SD
)
M
(
SD
)
M
(
SD
)
M
(
SD
)
M
(
SD
)
M
(
SD
)
12–15 (I)
3.82 (3.76)
4.56 (4.98)
1.82 (3.87)
9.87 (7.84)
23.80 (7.773)
23.76 (6.253)
17.61 (5.23)
16–20 (II)
1.18 (0.39)
1.52 (0.51)
0.15 (0.36)
1.85 (0.36)
19.91 (3.574)
25.30 (4.081)
19.61 (4.19)
21–29 (III)
1.11 (0.31)
1.70 (0.46)
0.00 (0.00)
2.20 (0.77)
22.82 (4.577)
25.11 (5.376)
17.70 (5.40)
30 and
older (IV)
1.36 (0.48)
2.50 (0.51)
0.00 (0.00)
2.78 (0.42)
22.08 (3.300)
24.54 (3.945)
16.04 (4.01)
ANOVA
F
(3,230) = 21,79
***
F
(3,230) = 13,33
***
F
(3,230) = 9,78
***
F
(3,230) = 42,60
***
F
(3,230) = 3,86
**
F
(3,230) = 1,10
**
F
(3,230) = 3,53
**
Post hoc
(
LSD
)
p
-value
<
0.001 Group I
>
2,3,4
<
0.001 Group
I
>
2,3,4
<
0.001 Group
I
>
2,3,4
<
0.001 Group
I
>
2,3,4
<
0.010 Group
I
>
II, and
group II
<
III
Non-significant
<
0.016 Group
I
<
II, and
group II
>
IV
M, mean; SD, standard deviation.
**
p
<
0.01,
***
p
<
0.001.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
5
March 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 429
Izydorczyk and Sitnik-Warchulska
Sociocultural Appearance Standards in Women
FIGURE 2 |
Statistics concerning the differences between the subgroups regarding the severity of sociocultural appearance standards.
TABLE 2 |
Summary of sociocultural tested predictors.
Dependent variable
Aged 12–15 (
n
= 95)
Aged 16–20 (
n
= 33)
Aged 21–29 (
n
= 56)
Aged 30 and older (
n
= 50)
Perfectionism
in
*
in
*
in
*
in
*
Pursuit of thinness
Adjusted
R
2
= 0.299
Adjusted
R
2
= 0.167
Adjusted
R
2
= 0.084
Adjusted
R
2
= 0.415
F
(3,91) = 14.36
***
F
(1,31) = 7.44
**
F
(1,54) = 6.08
**
F
(2,47) = 18.38
***
Internalization of
sociocultural norms
β
= 0.315
**
β
= 0.440
**
4
β
=
-
0.533
***
Pressure of
sociocultural norms
β
= 0.402
***
–
β
= 0.318
**
β
=
-
0.240
*
Bulimic tendencies
Adjusted
R
2
= 0,127
F
(1,91) = 14.63
***
in
*
in
*
in
*
Internalization of
sociocultural norms
β
= 0.369
***
Body dissatisfaction
Adjusted
R
2
= 0.239
F
(1,91) = 30.64
***
in
*
in
*
Adjusted
R
2
= 0.088
F
(1,48) = 5.74
*
Internalization of
sociocultural norms
β
= 0.498
***
β
= 0.327
*
in, insignificant.
*
p
<
0.05,
**
p
<
0.01,
***
p
<
0.001.
“risk factors for eating disorders.” Here, it can be seen that,
compared to the older adolescents and women participating
in this study, subgroup I showed the highest scores for body
dissatisfaction, along with the highest scores for relentless
pursuit of thinness and perfectionism. Further, the verified
constituents of the dependent variable (body dissatisfaction,
pursuit of thinness, bulimic tendencies, perfectionism) proved
to have the strongest effect on the youngest adolescent girls
when compared to the other participants (
p
<
0.001 for the
difference between subgroup I and subgroups II, III, and IV;
Table 1
). Meanwhile, compared to the severity of the other
aforementioned variables, the severity of bulimic tendencies
was lowest among the youngest adolescents. However, the
youngest girls obtained much higher values on the scale of
perfectionism
than
the
subjects
from
the
other
subgroups
(
Figure 1
). Moreover, this high score for perfectionism represents
a significant trait that differentiates subgroup I from all of the
other females, who scored equally on average (
Figure 1
and
Table 1
).
Characteristics of the Independent
Variable in the Participants
The results also implied that the greatest significant difference
in regard to the predictor variable was between subgroup I
and all other participants (
Figure 2
). Detailed analysis indicated
that this effect was mainly due to differences in average values
for the internalization and pressure variables (
Table 1
and
Figure 2
). The youngest girls showed much higher values on the
scale of internalization, especially in relation to late adolescents.
Girls aged 16–20 showed the lowest level of assimilation of
sociocultural appearance standards. Concurrently, the analyses
indicated that the youngest girls experienced less pressure
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
6
March 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 429
Your preview ends here
Eager to read complete document? Join bartleby learn and gain access to the full version
- Access to all documents
- Unlimited textbook solutions
- 24/7 expert homework help
Izydorczyk and Sitnik-Warchulska
Sociocultural Appearance Standards in Women
from various media messages than girls in late adolescence.
Meanwhile, women in middle adulthood showed the lowest
level of sociocultural pressure. The variable “exposure to body
images
promoted
in
mass
media”
did
not
differ
between
groups.
Characteristics of the Sociocultural
Predictors of Eating Disorders
Statistical analyses and regression models for the tested variables
showed that sociocultural appearance standards have a varied
impact on the development of the risk factors for eating disorders.
Based on the obtained adjusted
R
2
and beta (
β
) coefficient
values, presented in
Table 2
, one can conclude that only
seven models for the sociocultural appearance standards variable
explain
the
significant
impact
internalization
and
pressure
have on selected constituents of the variables for risk factors
for eating disorders (body dissatisfaction, pursuit of thinness,
bulimic tendencies). Together with the Fisher test results and
coefficients estimated based on the said results (at
p
<
0.001),
the models presented in
Table 2
were found to be statistically
significant.
In regard to summarizing the significant regression models,
one ought to note that, in subgroup I, variables associated with
the various risk factors for eating disorders (body dissatisfaction
and
pursuit
of
thinness)
were
most
strongly
affected
by
sociocultural appearance standards. Further, among the verified
constituents of the sociocultural appearance standards variable,
internalization (in groups I, II, IV) and pressure (in groups I, III,
IV) proved to be significant.
Among the entire study group, sociocultural appearance
standards
were
determined
to
be
predictors
of
the
pursuit
of
thinness,
regardless
of
age
and
BMI
values.
The
obtained
values
suggest
that
the
pursuit
of
thinness
variable is positively correlated with the pressure associated
with
various
media
messages
(in
girls
aged
12–15
and
women aged 21–29) and the internalization of appearance
standards
(in
girls
aged
12–15
and
those
aged
16–20).
Moreover,
the
conducted
analysis
indicated
that
women
in
middle
adulthood
engage
in
a
stronger
pursuit
of
thinness, provided they have less internalized sociocultural
appearance standards and experience less sociocultural pressure
(trend).
The
second
most
significant
variable
explained
by
the
internalization of sociocultural standards is body dissatisfaction.
The regression analysis conducted for the body dissatisfaction
variable in subgroup I and subgroup IV revealed high levels of
assimilation of appearance standards (internalization) to be a
significant predictor.
In the case of bulimic tendencies, only internalization of
sociocultural norms provided a significant explanation of this
tendency in subgroup I. In the other subgroups, the sociocultural
impact did not provide a significant explanation for bulimic
tendencies.
No significant forward stepwise regression models were
identified in the measurement of the impact exerted by the
sociocultural
appearance
standards
on
the
development
of
perfectionism among the participants.
DISCUSSION
The present research proves the existence of a relationship
between the occurrence of risk factors for eating disorders
(body dissatisfaction, pursuit of thinness, bulimic tendencies)
and
the
sociocultural
appearance
standards
(internalization
and pressure), promoted in mass media, in the studied girls
and women. The results of conducted research also indicate
that the relationship between sociocultural standards of the
appearance and risk factors of eating disorders is specific to the
developmental period.
In particular, the findings revealed that the youngest Polish
girls reported the highest level of risk factors for eating disorders.
Girls aged 12–15 were found to be the least satisfied with
their own bodies; meanwhile, compared to older adolescents
and young and mature women, the youngest participants also
reported significantly higher levels of efforts to become thin,
bulimic tendencies, and perfectionism. These results concerning
the young adolescent girls support research conducted by other
authors (
Stice et al., 2008
;
˙
Zechowski, 2008
;
Izydorczyk and
Rybicka-Klimczyk, 2009
;
Striegel-Moore et al., 2009
;
Brytek-
Matera and Rybicka-Klimczyk, 2012
;
McCabe et al., 2012
;
Izydorczyk, 2015a,b
); most of these previous studies focused
on body dissatisfaction and the pursuit of thinness. Meanwhile,
Ata et al. (2007)
indicated that as many as 70% of adolescent
girls would like to have a smaller body size. On a related
note, many studies have reported that dissatisfaction with
appearance is more prevalent among adolescent girls than
adolescent boys (
Neumark-Sztainer et al., 2002
;
McCabe and
Ricciardelli, 2004
;
Ata et al., 2007
;
Xu et al., 2010
;
Lawler
and Nixon, 2011
;
McCabe et al., 2012
); girls seem to be
more interested in losing weight, in contrast to boys, who
have
a
greater
desire
to
increase
muscle
(
Mellor
et
al.,
2009
).
The body dissatisfaction and pathological pursuit of thinness
identified among the youngest adolescents might be motivated
by psychosexual development processes and self-esteem based
on the principle “I am what I look like.” Furthermore, the said
life stage is characterized by body-image evaluation (particularly
among peers, but also parents) adopting an increased role in
social environments.
McCabe and Ricciardelli (2003)
indicated
the most significant influences for these factors; however, clinical
observations have indicated that mass media is becoming an
increasingly important part of the lives of today’s children
and adolescents (
Comstock and Scharrer, 2007
;
Hayes and
Tantleff-Dunn, 2010
;
Lamb and Peterson, 2012
). According
to
Gajtkowska (2013)
, in teenagers’ opinions, the media are
currently
the
most
common
source
of
information
about
appearance (78.67%).
However,
Field et al. (2001)
, in an examination of a group
of over 6,900 girls aged 9–15 years, obtained contrasting results.
Here, their analyses did not clearly show that there was a
correlation between the media (frequency of reading women’s
fashion magazines, use of diets to achieve a weight consistent with
common standards) and dissatisfaction with appearance. Thus, it
seems that exposure to mass media is not a sufficient explanatory
factor; the assimilation of sociocultural standards may also play a
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
7
March 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 429
Izydorczyk and Sitnik-Warchulska
Sociocultural Appearance Standards in Women
key role. This hypothesis is confirmed by the results of the present
research.
The regression analysis of the body-image dissatisfaction
variable for subgroup I revealed the internalization of appearance
standards to be a significant predictor in this regard. The
greater the body-image dissatisfaction among the youngest
adolescent girls, the greater the internalization of sociocultural
norms regarding body-image standards. It is interesting to note
that internalization also proved to be the only predictor of
bulimic tendencies, and only for subgroup I. Pressure and the
internalization of sociocultural appearance standards were also
found to be significantly positively correlated with the pursuit
of thinness in this subgroup. Supporting this finding,
Botta
(2003)
,
Shroff and Thompson (2006)
, and
Mäkinen et al. (2012)
all found a similar dependence between the internalization of
sociocultural norms and body dissatisfaction among the youngest
girls. This could be explained by the fact that, at the beginning
of adolescence, girls are significantly affected by biological and
psychosocial processes. The age of 12–15 is a developmental
period, the purpose of which is to determine one’s own identity.
During this period, it is particularly important to define one’s self
and reconcile the private self and the public self. The youngest
adolescents are influenced by society to adjust to the new tasks
they must fulfill while concurrently playing their psychosocial
roles as teenagers (a daughter, a student, a girlfriend, a colleague,
a friend). Consequently, at this point in their development, it is
possible that they only develop their psychological reactions to
teenage angst, and do not endeavor to negate the presence of
image-related pressure exerted by social norms.
It was observed that the youngest and older adolescents
showed
significantly
different
levels
of
pressure
in
terms
of sociocultural norms regarding the binding standards of
appearance. In the analysis of the results for subgroup II, the
level of internalization of sociocultural norms was found to
be significantly lower than that for all other subgroups. These
results correspond with the higher levels of pressure experienced
by these participants as a result of sociocultural norms. It is
worth considering whether adolescents in the so-called “teenage
angst” stage (16–20 years) do not manifest a general negation of
the impact the environment can have on their behavior, which
would be reflected in their decisions to negate the social pressure
experienced.
Furthermore, subgroup I showed a level of pressure as a result
of sociocultural norms that was similar to that experienced by
subgroup III. The higher the level of pressure, the greater the
body dissatisfaction. It is worth noting that young women of this
age are usually beginning to fulfill numerous new psychosocial
roles that are imposed on them (university student, employee,
romantic partner, wife, mother, etc.); therefore, they can feel
similar social pressure to the youngest girls. Adolescents and
young women react in a particularly strong manner as a result
of the psychological conflicts experienced in their life stages
(identity crises, psychological separation, and individualization
contrasted by a strong need for social acceptance and self-esteem;
Clark and Tiggemann, 2008
;
Izydorczyk and Rybicka-Klimczyk,
2009
;
Ferguson K. et al., 2011
;
Mond et al., 2011
;
Izydorczyk,
2015a
).
Halliwell (2013)
suggests that showing appreciation to young
women protects them from negative environmental appearance
messages. On the other hand,
Bair et al. (2012)
indicated that
young adults use the Internet as their primary news source and,
for this reason, they are more likely to internalize appearance
standards; as a consequence, they are at a greater risk of engaging
in disordered eating (
Bair et al., 2012
).
The results obtained from women aged 30 and above are
particularly interesting. There is a lack of research on body image
and desire for thinness among adult women. Bulimic tendencies
among the adult participants (aged 30 and older) proved to be
insignificant; adult women also had the lowest average scores
over the entire study population regarding exposure frequency
to body images in mass media and regarding the experience of
pressure exerted by sociocultural norms. The regression analysis
also indicated an interesting correlation: the more intensified
the internalization of sociocultural appearance standards, the
less thinness is pursued among women aged 30 and older.
A similar trend can be observed between pursuit of thinness and
pressure from sociocultural appearance standards. These results
are surprising, especially because previous studies have indicated
a reverse relationship in this regard (
Dittmar and Howard, 2004
;
Kilpela et al., 2015
).
Kilpela
et
al.
(2015)
noticed
that
body
image
may
be
more complex for adult women than for younger women.
Aging-related
physiological
changes
shift
the
female
body
further away from the thin-young ideal; further, life priorities
and psychological factors also evolve with age. Pregnancy,
employment, household obligations, and child care are factors
that can protect self-image (see
Kilpela et al., 2015
). Additionally,
according to
Fredrickson and Roberts (1997)
, middle-aged
women may experience decreased objectification as they age.
These results could be also explained by the fact that the life
stage of over 30 years is characterized by higher psychological
and social maturity. The women of this age in the present study
underwent upbringing and performed socialization for many
years under the cultural conditions of a highly industrialized
country, Poland. Given the fact that their personalities were
well structured and that they reported no mental disorders,
it can be stated that mature women are capable of opposing
the excessive sociocultural influences that promotes unhealthy
behavior, e.g., an overly thin body image. On the other hand, the
oldest of the participants were born and grew up in times when
access to the media was not so common. Perhaps, they have not
assimilated the sociocultural standards of appearance promoted
in contemporary media.
Perfectionism proved not to be affected by the sociocultural
impact of mass media.
Bardone-Cone et al. (2017)
highlighted
maladaptive perfectionism as a complex structure related to both
disordered eating and anxiety; meanwhile,
Petersson et al. (2017)
claimed that it is important to investigate patients’ definitions of
perfectionism, which the psychometric measures do not reflect.
The present study was performed on a Polish population of
girls and women. The results of the SATAQ-3 scale obtained in
our study seem to be slightly lower in terms of internalization and
exposure, compared to the results achieved by respondents from
other European countries (
Argyrides, 2013
). This observation
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
8
March 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 429
Izydorczyk and Sitnik-Warchulska
Sociocultural Appearance Standards in Women
should
be
treated
with
great
caution.
SATAQ
scales,
used
in
different
countries,
have
different
versions
and
validations.
However,
observations
in
other
countries
indicate
that
individuals who invest more time in their appearance, who have
internalized the thin ideal, who feel pressured by the media to
look a certain way, and who consider the media as a good source
of information for body-image issues are significantly more likely
to develop disordered eating behavior and patterns (
Argyrides,
2013
). The results conform with those of a study that surveyed
7,434 individuals in 10 major world regions in regard to body-
weight ideals and body dissatisfaction, which indicated that body
dissatisfaction is connected to Western media exposure (
Swami
et al., 2010
). Our study also supports a previous study featuring
a cross-western model, which contained a similar body ideal
(
Rodgers et al., 2011
); such an internalized appearance standard
can lead to body dissatisfaction and strengthen the pursuit of
thinness and disordered eating. Further, the interesting results we
obtained concerning adult women may also reflect the cultural
roles of women in southern Poland; however, this issue requires
further research. These findings also have important implications
for those seeking to prevent eating disorders through the
implementation of measures that are tailored to individual age
groups.
Limitations and Future Directions
The
conducted
study
is
characterized
by
some
limitations
pertaining to both the sampling method and the research
procedure. Firstly, the study participants (despite being selected
in accordance with the research objective and the required
procedure)
might
have
constituted
a
specific
sample
of
adolescent girls and women with a specific background, and
consequently the conclusions obtained from the study findings
are likely not to be applicable to the remaining population.
Nevertheless, the use of a common socio-demographic criterion
for sampling and the number of participants support the
validity of the conducted study. Secondly, in order to investigate
the dynamics of the psychological processes and motivations
that
drive
body-focused
behavior,
particularly
concerning
factors that generate body-image distortions typical of eating
disorders, longitudinal studies are required, as these would
ensure higher reliability and accuracy in the evaluation of
the
research
material
and
would
also
allow
results
to
be
compared either over several years or over a brief period,
when the participants are contacted by the researchers. In
fact, a long-term longitudinal study on a group of females
would ensure a more in-depth measurement of the processes
underlying the development of negative attitudes toward the
body. However, considering the time-consuming nature of
such studies and the low possibility that such studies can be
conducted, as a result of difficulties accessing study groups
and developing appropriate research procedures, this form
of procedure was not pursued in the present study. Finally,
given the specified research objectives and the procedure, as
well as the measurement methods presented in the literature,
it
seems
likely
that
the
adopted
assumption
and
research
procedure
can
be
implemented
by
means
of
transversal
studies.
CONCLUSION
Young adolescent girls constitute a high-risk group in regard
to possessing a specific psychological proneness to developing
eating
disorders
as
a
result
of
the
sociocultural
influence
exerted by mass media. Two main sociocultural predictors
(internalization
and
pressure
of
sociocultural
norms)
play
a
significant
role
in
explaining
the
development
of
body
dissatisfaction and the pathological pursuit of thinness among
Polish adolescent girls and women. In relation to perfectionism,
no significant role of internalization and sociocultural pressure
was observed. Meanwhile, in women aged 30 and over, the high
level of internalization of sociocultural appearance standards
seems to be significantly linked to body satisfaction. In the
future, intercultural research would be interesting, especially
studies focusing on women in middle adulthood. The obtained
study results can prove helpful for creating education programs
in preventive healthcare aimed particularly at the youngest
adolescents.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
BI made substantial contributions to the conception of the work
and to the interpretation of data. BI and KS-W made substantial
contributions to the design of the work and the interpretation
of data and to drafting the work and revising it critically for
important intellectual content. Both authors gave final approval
of the version to be published and agreed to be accountable for all
aspects of the work.
REFERENCES
Argyrides, M. (2013).
On the Cover: Media Influences, Body Image and Disordered
Eating in European Youth
. Brussels: European Confederation of Youth Clubs.
Ata, R. N., Ludden, A. B., and Lally, M. M. (2007). The effect of garden
and family, friend and media influences on eatingbehaviors and body image
duringadolescence.
J. Youth Adolesc.
36, 1024–1037. doi: 10.1007/s10964-006-
9159-x
Bair, C. E., Kelly, N. R., Serdar, K. L., and Mazzeo, S. E. (2012). Does
the Internet function like magazines? An exploration of image-focused
media, eating pathology, and body dissatisfaction.
Eat. Behav.
13, 398–401.
doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2012.06.003
Bardone-Cone, A. M., Lin, S. L., and Butler, R. M. (2017). Perfectionism and
contingent self-worth in relation to disordered eating and anxiety.
Behav. Ther.
48, 380–390. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2016.05.006
Bearman, S. K., Presnell, K., and Martinez, E. (2006). The skinny on body
dissatisfaction: a longitudinalstudy of adolescent girls and boys.
J. Youth
Adolesc.
35, 217–229. doi: 10.1007/s10964-005-9010-9
Benton, C., and Karazsia, B. T. (2015). The effect of thin and muscular images on
women’s body satisfaction.
Body Image
13, 22–27. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.
11.001
Botta, R. A. (2003). For your health? The relationship between magazine reading
and adolescents’ body image and eating disturbances.
Sex Roles
48, 389–399.
doi: 10.1023/A:1023570326812
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
9
March 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 429
Your preview ends here
Eager to read complete document? Join bartleby learn and gain access to the full version
- Access to all documents
- Unlimited textbook solutions
- 24/7 expert homework help
Izydorczyk and Sitnik-Warchulska
Sociocultural Appearance Standards in Women
Brytek-Matera,
A.,
and
Rybicka-Klimczyk,
A.
(2012).
Ocena
nasilenia
objawów
syndromu
gotowo´
sci
anorektycznej
u
mlodych
kobiet
-
badania
pilota˙zowe
[Assessment
of
anorexia
readiness
syndrome
escalating symptomsin young women – a pilot study].
Stud. Psychol.
12,
23–26.
Cash, T. F. (2011). “Crucial considerations in the assessment of body image,” in
Body Image: A Handbook of Science, Practice, and Prevention
, eds T. F. Cash
and L. Smolak (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 129–137.
Clark, L., and Tiggemann, M. (2008). Sociocultural and individual psychological
predictors of body image in young girls: a prospective study.
Dev. Psychol.
44,
1124–1134. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.4.1124
Comstock, G., and Scharrer, E. (2007).
Media and the American Child
. Burlington,
MA: Academic Press.
Czepczor, K., Ko´
scicka, K., and Brytek-Matera, A. (2016). Spoleczno-kulturowe
postawy wobec wlasnego wygla
¸du i niezadowolenie z ciala u kobiet i me
¸˙zczyzn
w okresie pó´znej adolescencji: badania wstêpne [The sociocultural attitudes
towards appearance and body dissatisfaction among late adolescence: a pilot
study].
Pol. Forum Psychol.
21, 364–377. doi: 10.14656/PFP20160303
Dittmar, H., and Howard, S. (2004). Thin-ideal internalization and social
comparison tendency as moderators of media models’ impance on women’s
body-focused anxiety.
J. Soc. Clin. Psychol.
23, 768–791. doi: 10.1521/jscp.23.
6.768.54799
Ferguson, K., Munoz, M. E., Contreras, S., and Velasquez, K. (2011). Mirror,
mirror on the wall: peer competition, television influences, and body image
dissatisfaction.
J. Soc. Clin. Psychol.
30, 458–483. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2011.30.
5.458
Ferguson, C. J., Winegard, B., and Winegard Bo, M. (2011). Who is the fairest
one of all? How evolution guides peer and media influences on female body
dissatisfaction.
Rev. Gen. Psychol.
15, 11–28. doi: 10.1037/a0022607
Field, A., Camargo, C. A., Taylor, C. B., Barkey, C. S., Roberts, S., and Colditz,
G. A. (2001). Peer, parent and media influences on the development of weight
concerns and frequent dieting among preadolescent and adolescent girls and
boys.
Pediatrics
107, 54–60. doi: 10.1542/peds.107.1.54
Finne, E., Bucksch, J., Lampert, T., and Kolip, P. (2011). Age, puberty, body
dissatisfaction and psychical activity decline in adolescents. Results of German
Health Interview and examination survey (KIGGS).
Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys.
Act.
8, 119–133. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-8-119
Fredrickson, B. L., and Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory.
Psychol.
Women Q.
21, 173–206. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1997.tb00108.x
Gajtkowska, M. (2013). Obraz wlasnego ciala wspoìlczesnej mlodziez?y a kultura
popularna. Badania wlasne [Self-image Body of Contemporary Youth and Pop
Culture, Own research].
Kult. Spolecz. Edukacja
2, 103–118. doi: 10.14746/kse.
2013.4.2.07
Garner, D. M. (2004).
EDI-3. Eating Disorders Inventory
. Lutz, FL: Psychological
Assessment Resources, Inc.
Glogowska, J., and Zatorska, A. (2016). Wizerunek ciala dziewcza
¸t w wieku 8 – 9
lat [Body image of girls aged 8 – 9 years].
Zest Nauk. WSKFiT
11, 29–34.
Grabe, S., Ward, L., and Hyde, J. (2008). The role of the media in body image
concerns among women: a meta-analysis of experimental and correlational
studies.
Psychol. Bull.
134, 460–476. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.460
Halliwell, H. (2013). The impact of thin idealized media images on body
satisfaction: does body appreciation protect women from negative effects?
Body
Image
10, 509–514. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.07.004
Hayes, S., and Tantleff-Dunn, S. (2010). Am I too fat to be a princess? Examining
the effects of popular children’s media on young girls’ body image.
Br. J. Dev.
Psychol.
28(Pt 2), 413–426. doi: 10.1348/026151009X424240
Izydorczyk,
B.
(2014).
Postawy
i
Zachowania
Wobec
Wlasnego
Ciala
w
Zaburzeniach Od˙zywiania [Attitudes and Behavior Towards One’s Own Body in
Eating Disorders]
. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN.
Izydorczyk, B. (2015a). Psychological and socio-cultural risk factors for developing
negative attitude and anti-health behavior toward the body in young women.
Polish Psychol. Bull.
46, 555–572. doi: 10.1515/ppb-2015-0062
Izydorczyk, B. (2015b). A psychological typology of females diagnosed with
anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa or binge eating disorder.
Health Psychol. Rep.
3, 312–325. doi: 10.5114/hpr.2015.55169
Izydorczyk, B., and Rybicka-Klimczyk, A. (2009). Poznawcze aspekty obrazu ciala
u kobiet a zaburzenia od˙zywiania.
[Cognitive aspects of women’s body image and
eating disorders]
.
Endokrynol. Pol.
60, 287–294.
Jones, D. C., and Crawford, J. K. (2006). The peer appearance culture during
adolescence: gender and body mass variations.
J. Youth Adolesc.
35, 257–269.
doi: 10.1007/s10964-005-9006-5
Józefik, B. (2014).
). Kultura, Cialo, (nie) Jedzenie. Terapia. Perspektywa Narracyjno
- Konstrukcjonistyczna w Zaburzeniach Od˙zywiania [Culture, body, (not)
food. Therapy. Narrative-constructivist perspective on eating disorders]
. Kraków:
Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Jagiello´nskiego.
Kilpela, L. S., Becker, C. B., Wesley, N., and Stewart, T. (2015). Body Image in Adult
Women: Moving Beyond the Younger Years.
Adv. Eat. Disord.
3, 144–164.
doi: 10.1080/21662630.2015.1012728
Knauss, C., Paxton, S. J., and Alsaker, F. D. (2009). Validation of the German
version
of
the
sociocultural
attitudes
towards
appearance
questionnaire
(SATAQ-G).
Body Image
6, 113–120. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.01.002
Lamb, S., and Peterson, Z. (2012). Adolescent girls’ sexual empowerment: Two
feminists explore the concept.
Sex Roles
66, 703–712. doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-
9995-3
Lawler, M., and Nixon, E. (2011). Body dissatisfaction among adolescent boys
and girls: the effects of body mass, peer appearance culture and internalization
of appearance ideals.
J. Youth Adolesc.
40, 59–71. doi: 10.1007/s10964-009-
9500-2
Levine, M. P., and Murnen, S. K. (2009). Everybody knows that mass media
are/are not [pick one] a cause of eating disorders”: a critical review of
evidence for a causal link between media, negative body image, and disordered
eating in females.
J. Soc. Clin. Psychol.
28, 9–42. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2009.
28.1.9
Levine, M. P., and Smolak, L. (1996). “Media as a context for the development of
disordered eating,” in
The Developmental Psychopathology of Eating Disorders
,
eds L. Smolak, M. P. Levine, and R. Striegel-Moore (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum),
235–257.
Levine, M. P., and Smolak, L. (2006).
The Prevention of Eating Problems and Eating
Disorders: Theory, Research, and Practice
. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Levine, M. P., Smolak, L., and Hayden, H. (1994). The relation of sociocultural
factors to eating attitudes and behaviors among middle school girls.
J. Early
Adolesc.
14, 471–490. doi: 10.1177/0272431694014004004
Mäkinen, M., Pukko-Viertomies, L.-R., Lindberg, N., Siimes, M. A., and Alberg, V.
(2012). Body dissatisfaction and body mass in girls and boys transitioning from
early to mid adolescence: additional role of self-esteem and eating habits.
BMC
Psychiatry
12:35. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-35
McCabe, M., and Ricciardelli, L. A. (2004). A longitudinal study of pubertal
timing and extreme body change behaviors among adolescent boys and girls.
Adolescence
39, 145–166.
McCabe, M., and Ricciardelli, L. A. (2009). Extreme weight change behaviours: are
overweight and normal weight adolescents different, and does this vary over
time?
Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev.
17, 301–314. doi: 10.1002/erv.929
McCabe, M. P., Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M., Mellor, D., Ricciardelli, L., Skouteris, H.,
and
Mussap,
A.
(2012).
Body
satisfaction
among
adolescents
in
eight
different countries.
J. Health Psychol.
17, 693–701. doi: 10.1177/135910531142
5274
McCabe, M. P., and Ricciardelli, L. A. (2003). Sociocultural influences on body
image and body changes among adolescent boys and girls.
J. Soc. Psychol.
143,
5–26. doi: 10.1080/00224540309598428
Mellor, D., McCabe, M., Ricciardelli, L., Yeow, J., Daliza, N., and Hapidzal,
N. F. (2009). Sociocultural influences on body dissatisfaction and body change
behaviors among Malaysian adolescents.
Body Image
6, 121–128. doi: 10.1016/
j.bodyim.2008.11.003
Menzel, J. E., Sperry, S. L., Small, B., Thompson, J. K., Sarwer, D. B., and Cash,
T. F. (2011). Internalization of appearance ideals and cosmetic surgery attitudes:
a test of the tripartite influence model of body image.
Sex Roles
65, 469–477.
doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-9983-7
Mond, J., Van den Berg, P., Boutelle, K., Hannan, P., and Neumark-Sztainer, D.
(2011). Obesity, body dissatisfaction, and emotional well-being in early and late
adolescence: finding from the project EAT study.
J. Adolesc. Health
48, 373–378.
doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.07.022
Neumark-Sztainer, D., Story, M., Hannan, P. J., Perry, C., and Irving, L. M. (2002).
Weight-related concerns and behaviors among overweight and non-overweight
adolescents: implications for preventing weight-related disorders.
Arch. Pediatr.
Adolesc. Med.
156, 171–178. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.156.2.171
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
10
March 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 429
Izydorczyk and Sitnik-Warchulska
Sociocultural Appearance Standards in Women
Nielson, H. E., Reel, J. J., Galli, N. A., Crookston, B. T., and Miyairi, M. (2013). Body
image and westernization trends among Japanese adolescents.
Health Educ.
45,
4–10.
Petersson, S., Johnsson, P., and Perseius, K. I. (2017). A Sisyphean task:
experiences of perfectionism in patients with eating disorders.
J. Eat. Disord.
5:3.
doi: 10.1186/s40337-017-0136-4
Rodgers, R., Chabrol, H., and Paxton, S. J. (2011). An exploration of the tripartite
influence model of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating among Australian
and French college women.
Body Image
8, 208–215. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.
04.009
Rodgers, R. F. (2016). The role of the “Healthy Weight” discourse in body image
and eating concerns: an extension of sociocultural theory.
Eat. Behav.
22,
194–198. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.06.004
Sánchez-Carracedo, D., Barrada, J. R., López-Guimerà, G., Fauquet, J., Almenara,
C. A., and Trepat, E. (2012). Analysis of the factor structure of the sociocultural
attitudes towards appearance questionnaire (SATAQ-3) in Spanish secondary-
school students through exploratory structural equation modeling.
Body Image
8, 208–215. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.10.002
Shroff, H., and Thompson, J. K. (2006). The tripartite influence model of body
image and eating disturbance: a replication with adolescent girls.
Body Image
3, 17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.10.004
Stice, E., Killen, J. D., Hayward, C., and Taylor, C. B. (1998). Support for the
continuity hypothesis of bulimic pathology.
J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.
66,
784–790. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.66.5.784
Stice, E., Marti, C. N., and Rohde, P. (2013). Prevalence, incidence, impairment an
course of the proposed DSM-V eating disorder diagnoses in 8-year prospective
community study of young women.
J. Abnorom. Psychol.
122, 445–457.
doi: 10.1037/a0030679
Stice, E., Marti, C. N., Spoor, S., Presnell, K., and Shaw, H. (2008). Dissonance and
healthy weight eating disorder prevention programs: long-term effects from a
randomized efficacy trial.
J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.
76, 329–340. doi: 10.1037/
0022-006X.76.2.329
Stice, E., Schupak-Neuberg, E., Shaw, H. E., and Stein, R. I. (1994). Relation
of media exposure to eating disorder symptomatology: a examination of
mediating mechanisms.
J. Abnorm. Psychol.
103, 836–840. doi: 10.1037/0021-
843X.103.4.836
Stice, E., and Whitenton, K. (2002). Risk factors for body dissatisfaction in
adolescent girls: a longitudinal investigation.
Dev. Psychol.
38, 669–678.
doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.38.5.669
Striegel-Moore, R. H., Roselli, F., Perrin, N., DeBar, L., Wilson, G. T., Mag, A., et al.
(2009). Gender diferrence in the prevalaence of eating disorder symptoms.
Int.
J. Eat. Disord.
42, 471–474. doi: 10.1002/eat.20625
Swami, V., Frederick, D. A., Aavik, T., Alcalay, L., Allik, J., Anderson, D., et al.
(2010). The attractive female body weight and female body dissatisfaction
in 26 countries across 10 world regions: results of the international body
project I.
Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull.
36, 309–325. doi: 10.1177/014616720935
9702
Thompson, K. J., Van den Berg, P., Roehrig, M., Guarda, A. S., and Heinberg,
L. J. (2004). The socjocultural attitudes towards appearance Scale-3 (SATAQ-3).
Development and Vaidation.
J. Eat. Disord.
35, 293–300. doi: 10.1002/eat.10257
Thomsen, S. R., Weber, M. M., and Brown, L. B. (2002). The relationship between
reading beauty and fashion magazines and the use of pathogenic dieting
methods among adolescent females.
Adolescence
37, 1–18.
Tiggemann, M. (2003). Media exposure, body dissatisfaction and disordered
eating: television and magazines are not the same.
Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev.
11,
418–430. doi: 10.1002/erv.502
Wilcox, K., and Laird, J. D. (2000). The impact of media images of super-
slender women and women’s self-esteem: Identification, social comparison, and
self-perception.
J. Res. Pers.
34, 278–286. doi: 10.1006/jrpe.1999.2281
Xu, X., Mellor, D., Kiehne, M., Ricciardelli, L. A., McCabe, M. P., and Xu, Y. (2010).
Body dissatisfaction, engagement in body change behaviors and sociocultural
influences on body image among Chinese adolescents.
Body Image
7, 156–164.
doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.11.003
˙
Zechowski, C. (2008). Polska wersja Kwestionariusza Zaburzenì Od˙zywiania
(EDI) – adaptacja i normalizacja [Polish Version of Eating Disorder Inventory –
adaptation and normalization].
Psychiatr. Pol.
42, 179–193.
Conflict of Interest Statement:
The authors declare that the research was
conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2018 Izydorczyk and Sitnik-Warchulska. This is an open-access article
distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY).
The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the
original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original
publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice.
No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these
terms.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
11
March 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 429
Related Documents
Recommended textbooks for you

Ciccarelli: Psychology_5 (5th Edition)
Psychology
ISBN:9780134477961
Author:Saundra K. Ciccarelli, J. Noland White
Publisher:PEARSON

Cognitive Psychology
Psychology
ISBN:9781337408271
Author:Goldstein, E. Bruce.
Publisher:Cengage Learning,

Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and ...
Psychology
ISBN:9781337565691
Author:Dennis Coon, John O. Mitterer, Tanya S. Martini
Publisher:Cengage Learning

Psychology in Your Life (Second Edition)
Psychology
ISBN:9780393265156
Author:Sarah Grison, Michael Gazzaniga
Publisher:W. W. Norton & Company

Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research a...
Psychology
ISBN:9781285763880
Author:E. Bruce Goldstein
Publisher:Cengage Learning

Theories of Personality (MindTap Course List)
Psychology
ISBN:9781305652958
Author:Duane P. Schultz, Sydney Ellen Schultz
Publisher:Cengage Learning
Recommended textbooks for you
- Ciccarelli: Psychology_5 (5th Edition)PsychologyISBN:9780134477961Author:Saundra K. Ciccarelli, J. Noland WhitePublisher:PEARSONCognitive PsychologyPsychologyISBN:9781337408271Author:Goldstein, E. Bruce.Publisher:Cengage Learning,Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and ...PsychologyISBN:9781337565691Author:Dennis Coon, John O. Mitterer, Tanya S. MartiniPublisher:Cengage Learning
- Psychology in Your Life (Second Edition)PsychologyISBN:9780393265156Author:Sarah Grison, Michael GazzanigaPublisher:W. W. Norton & CompanyCognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research a...PsychologyISBN:9781285763880Author:E. Bruce GoldsteinPublisher:Cengage LearningTheories of Personality (MindTap Course List)PsychologyISBN:9781305652958Author:Duane P. Schultz, Sydney Ellen SchultzPublisher:Cengage Learning

Ciccarelli: Psychology_5 (5th Edition)
Psychology
ISBN:9780134477961
Author:Saundra K. Ciccarelli, J. Noland White
Publisher:PEARSON

Cognitive Psychology
Psychology
ISBN:9781337408271
Author:Goldstein, E. Bruce.
Publisher:Cengage Learning,

Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and ...
Psychology
ISBN:9781337565691
Author:Dennis Coon, John O. Mitterer, Tanya S. Martini
Publisher:Cengage Learning

Psychology in Your Life (Second Edition)
Psychology
ISBN:9780393265156
Author:Sarah Grison, Michael Gazzaniga
Publisher:W. W. Norton & Company

Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research a...
Psychology
ISBN:9781285763880
Author:E. Bruce Goldstein
Publisher:Cengage Learning

Theories of Personality (MindTap Course List)
Psychology
ISBN:9781305652958
Author:Duane P. Schultz, Sydney Ellen Schultz
Publisher:Cengage Learning
Browse Popular Homework Q&A
Q: The two methods of expressing bearing can be interpreted using a rectangular coordinate system.…
Q: yare Corporation had the following inventory balances at the beginning and end of May:
May…
Q: ] = ²P (£ + 29 + +) 7
Find(+6e+3) dz
Q: Jared Diamond places a large emphasis on pre-existing biological and zoological factors in the…
Q: Can you also do three-eight??
Q: Testing for a disease can be made more efficient by combining samples. If the samples from six…
Q: In conditions of dehydration, plant cells can increase their water retention by regulating the…
Q: Which of the following provides the most likely explanation for the limiting factor of a
cell's…
Q: 12.
d) noltionos le
ter Wonsmo
color-blind
A red-green
man (sex-linked gene) marries a normal
woman.…
Q: Apply the concept of the supply chain to Progressive.
Q: Which statement correctly explains the formation of hydrogen bonds?
A
B
с
D
The partially negative…
Q: Define the Fisher effect. To what extent do empirical tests confirm that the Fisher effect exists in…
Q: What are some ways that social proof can help a leader succeed? What are some ways that social proof…
Q: Toma and Kana can produce either books or shoes. If they spent all of their time producing one
of…
Q: A frequency table of grades has five classes (A, B, C, D, F) with frequencies of 3, 12, 18, 7, and 1…
Q: History and background of the company QVC?
Q: 1
f(x) = -1
(a) State the domain of the function
(b) Identify all intercepts
(c) Find any vertical…
Q: Decide whether the Lewis structure proposed for each molecule is reasonable or not.
molecule…
Q: what are the six paradigms of human interaction and why the paradigm of win/win is most important in…
Q: Evaluate the indefinite integral.
tan^-1 (8x)/(1+64x^2) dx
Q: Asssume that the data below is normally distributed and a researcher wants to know at the 4% level…
Q: What significance does the relative size of exons and introns have for the evolution of genes and of…
Q: Two classes take a test. At the .05 level the researcher wants to know if the first class has a…
Q: Is the following molecule polar or nonpolar
Polar
Nonpolar
SIOCI 2
Q: Compute the given integral.
5x dr
+C