Read the research article added here and answer the question: 1. What type of variable is conception risk status? Group of answer choices A. Quasi-dependent variable B. Quasi-independent variable C. Predictor variable D. Outcome variable E. Independent variable F. Dependent variable

Ciccarelli: Psychology_5 (5th Edition)
5th Edition
ISBN:9780134477961
Author:Saundra K. Ciccarelli, J. Noland White
Publisher:Saundra K. Ciccarelli, J. Noland White
Chapter1: The Science Of Psychology
Section: Chapter Questions
Problem 1TY
icon
Related questions
Question
Read the research article added here and answer the question: 1. What type of variable is conception risk status? Group of answer choices A. Quasi-dependent variable B. Quasi-independent variable C. Predictor variable D. Outcome variable E. Independent variable F. Dependent variable
Research Report
Women Are More Likely to Wear Red
or Pink at Peak Fertility
Alec T. Beall and Jessica L. Tracy
University of British Columbia
aps
DATION FOR
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE
Sexual intercourse typically results in conception only
within the few days of a woman's cycle prior to and dur-
ing ovulation (Wilcox, Weinberg, & Baird, 1995), making
it potentially adaptive for women to advertise their fertile
window in an observable manner that attracts male atten-
tion, as do females of closely related primate species
(Domb & Pagel, 2001; see Haselton & Gildersleeve, 2011;
but also see Burley, 1979). However, scientists have not
found any clearly observable, objective behavioral dis-
play associated with ovulation in humans. Building on
evidence that women are motivated to enhance their
attractiveness during ovulation (Haselton, Mortezaie,
Pillsworth, Bleske-Rechek, & Frederick, 2007), and that
men perceive shades of red-when associated with
women-as sexually attractive (Elliot & Niesta, 2008), we
tested whether women preferentially display these colors
during peak fertility.
Psychological Science
249) 1857-1841
The Author(s) 2015
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions nav
DOI 10.1177/0956797613476045
Abstract
Although females of many species closely related to humans signal their fertile window in an observable manner,
often involving red or pink coloration, no such display has been found for humans. Building on evidence that men are
sexually attracted to women wearing or surrounded by red, we tested whether women show a behavioral tendency
toward wearing reddish clothing when at peak fertility. Across two samples (N=124), women at high conception risk
were more than 3 times more likely to wear a red or pink shirt than were women at low conception risk, and 77%
of women who wore red or pink were found to be at high, rather than low, risk. Conception risk had no effect on
the prevalence of any other shirt color. Our results thus suggest that red and pink adornment in women is reliably
associated with fertility and that female ovulation, long assumed to be hidden, is associated with a salient visual cue.
Individuals across cultures associate red with love and
passion (Aslam, 2006). Studies using a range of methods
and populations have demonstrated that women's use of
red is linked to sex and romance (e.g., Elliot & Pazda,
2012; Greenfield, 2005) and that men find women wear-
ing or surrounded by red particularly attractive and
pss.sagepub.com
SSAGE
Keywords
fertility, human mate selection, color perception, conception risk, ovulation, evolutionary psychology, social
cognition, physical appearance
Received 5/4/12; Revision accepted 1/5/13
sexually desirable (Elliot & Niesta, 2008). This last effect
has been demonstrated among members of a highly iso-
lated, traditional, small-scale society where red carries
cultural associations divergent from those found in the
West, suggesting that the link between red and sexual
attraction may be universal (Elliot, Tracy, Pazda, & Beall,
2013).
Several possible explanations for men's attraction to
redness on or surrounding women have been proffered
(see Elliot & Niesta, 2008)-including, for example, red's
attention-grabbing perceptual properties, the fact that
reddish skin tone often indicates sexual arousal in
women, and the finding that males of other species show
a similar attraction (Bielert, Girolami, & Jowell, 1989),
suggesting that these cognitions might have originally
emerged in a shared ancestor and have been retained in
humans. Regardless of the mechanism underlying this
Corresponding Author:
Alec T. Beall, Department of Psychology, University of British
Columbia, 2136 West Mall, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z4,
Canada
E-mail: alec@psych.ubc.ca
1838
attraction, the well-documented presence of this ten-
dency among men leads to an interesting prediction
regarding women. Given women's brief fertile window, it
may be adaptive for them to dress in a way that increases
their sexual attractiveness to men during this period.
Women thus may self-adorn in red or pink during this
time as part of a broader drive to appear more sexually
attractive at peak fertility (Durante, Li, & Haselton, 2008;
Haselton & Gangestad, 2006). Alternatively, if the hor-
mones associated with ovulation make women feel
sexier during peak fertility (Haselton & Gangestad, 2006),
they may be inclined to wear clothing with attention-
grabbing properties (e.g., red-colored clothing) during
this time, even in the absence of any desire to attract
men.
Despite these possibilities, prior research has sug-
gested that the advertisement of human female fertility is
largely hidden, involving vocal, olfactory, and subtle
visual changes, but few overt behavioral displays (Bryant
& Haselton, 2008; Miller & Maner, 2011). For example,
although two studies have suggested that women's faces
are judged as more attractive during peak fertility, the
women in these studies posed neutral expressions and
removed all cosmetics prior to being photographed,
making it unlikely that their increased attractiveness was
due to any objectively measureable behavioral display
(Puts et al., 2013; Roberts et al., 2004).
Other studies have documented changes in women's
mate-seeking desires and behavioral tendencies during
peak fertility: a self-reported increased desire to have sex
with men (e.g., Haselton & Gangestad, 2006), an increased
attraction to physical markers of health and masculinity
(Penton-Voak & Perrett, 2000), an increased desire to
wear revealing clothing (Durante et al., 2008), and a ten-
dency to wear clothing that leads women to be judged as
trying to look more attractive (Haselton et al., 2007). In
addition, one study found that women at peak fertility
wore more revealing clothing, but this effect emerged
only among partnered women (whose partners were
absent) attending Austrian discotheques, where, presum-
ably, dressing provocatively does not violate social norms
(Grammer, Renninger, & Fischer, 2004). These caveats are
important because, despite several attempts, prior studies
have thus far failed to document a generalized, objec-
tively observable change in women's everyday behavior
or dress that is linked to ovulation.
Furthermore, although Haselton, Mortezaie, Pillsworth,
Bleske-Rechek, and Frederick (2007) found that part-
nered women were rated as dressing more fashionably at
peak fertility, there was no difference in ratings of sexi-
ness (and, again, the effect on fashionableness was
restricted to partnered women). Similarly, although
Durante and colleagues (2008; Durante, Griskevicius,
Hill, Perilloux, & Li, 2011) found that women at peak
fertility reported a desire to purchase and wear sexier
Beall, Tracy
clothing when imagining attending a social gathering at
which they might meet men, no difference emerged in
the observed sexiness of the clothing the women actually
wore. Durante and colleagues (2008) argued that experi-
mental studies may fail to document an ovulation-linked
change in women's dress because dressing in provoca-
tive clothing is often not socially acceptable, particularly
for women participating in research on a university cam-
pus. As a result, it remains unclear whether there is any
salient, observable behavioral display reliably associated
with female ovulation.
Building on the evidence reviewed above suggesting
that women may seek to increase their attractiveness by
self-adorning in reddish colors, and should be particu-
larly motivated to do so during peak fertility, we tested
whether women are more likely to wear red- or pink-
colored clothing during this period, compared with other
phases of their menstrual cycle. Support for this predic-
tion would provide the first evidence for a distinct and
visually obvious behavioral display linked to female
ovulation.
Method
Participants
Sample A. A total of 100 American women (aged 18-47
years, Mdn = 26) recruited through Amazon's Mechanical
Turk (see Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011) partici-
pated in return for monetary compensation.
Sample B. A total of 24 female undergraduates at the
University of British Columbia (aged 17-31 years, Mdn =
19) participated in exchange for course credit.
Procedure
Two samples of regularly ovulating female participants
completed measures online. Participants responded to
the question, "What color is the shirt you are currently
wearing? (If your shirt is multicolored, please select the
color which is most prevalent)." Response options were
"black," "blue," "gray," "green," "pink," "red," "white," "yel-
low," and "other." Participants also responded to the
question, "How many days has it been since the onset of
your last period of menses? Responses were used to
divide women into a high-conception-risk group (Days
6-14; Sample A: n = 51; Sample B: n = 10) and a low-
conception-risk group (Days 0-5 and 15-28; Sample A
n = 49, Sample B: n = 14) on the basis of a standard
28-day model of the menstrual cycle (Penton-Voak &
Perrett, 2000). We excluded women whose first day of
menses had occurred more than 28 days previously to
avoid including women experiencing atypical cycles
(Little, Jones, & DeBruine, 2008).²
Transcribed Image Text:Research Report Women Are More Likely to Wear Red or Pink at Peak Fertility Alec T. Beall and Jessica L. Tracy University of British Columbia aps DATION FOR PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Sexual intercourse typically results in conception only within the few days of a woman's cycle prior to and dur- ing ovulation (Wilcox, Weinberg, & Baird, 1995), making it potentially adaptive for women to advertise their fertile window in an observable manner that attracts male atten- tion, as do females of closely related primate species (Domb & Pagel, 2001; see Haselton & Gildersleeve, 2011; but also see Burley, 1979). However, scientists have not found any clearly observable, objective behavioral dis- play associated with ovulation in humans. Building on evidence that women are motivated to enhance their attractiveness during ovulation (Haselton, Mortezaie, Pillsworth, Bleske-Rechek, & Frederick, 2007), and that men perceive shades of red-when associated with women-as sexually attractive (Elliot & Niesta, 2008), we tested whether women preferentially display these colors during peak fertility. Psychological Science 249) 1857-1841 The Author(s) 2015 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions nav DOI 10.1177/0956797613476045 Abstract Although females of many species closely related to humans signal their fertile window in an observable manner, often involving red or pink coloration, no such display has been found for humans. Building on evidence that men are sexually attracted to women wearing or surrounded by red, we tested whether women show a behavioral tendency toward wearing reddish clothing when at peak fertility. Across two samples (N=124), women at high conception risk were more than 3 times more likely to wear a red or pink shirt than were women at low conception risk, and 77% of women who wore red or pink were found to be at high, rather than low, risk. Conception risk had no effect on the prevalence of any other shirt color. Our results thus suggest that red and pink adornment in women is reliably associated with fertility and that female ovulation, long assumed to be hidden, is associated with a salient visual cue. Individuals across cultures associate red with love and passion (Aslam, 2006). Studies using a range of methods and populations have demonstrated that women's use of red is linked to sex and romance (e.g., Elliot & Pazda, 2012; Greenfield, 2005) and that men find women wear- ing or surrounded by red particularly attractive and pss.sagepub.com SSAGE Keywords fertility, human mate selection, color perception, conception risk, ovulation, evolutionary psychology, social cognition, physical appearance Received 5/4/12; Revision accepted 1/5/13 sexually desirable (Elliot & Niesta, 2008). This last effect has been demonstrated among members of a highly iso- lated, traditional, small-scale society where red carries cultural associations divergent from those found in the West, suggesting that the link between red and sexual attraction may be universal (Elliot, Tracy, Pazda, & Beall, 2013). Several possible explanations for men's attraction to redness on or surrounding women have been proffered (see Elliot & Niesta, 2008)-including, for example, red's attention-grabbing perceptual properties, the fact that reddish skin tone often indicates sexual arousal in women, and the finding that males of other species show a similar attraction (Bielert, Girolami, & Jowell, 1989), suggesting that these cognitions might have originally emerged in a shared ancestor and have been retained in humans. Regardless of the mechanism underlying this Corresponding Author: Alec T. Beall, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, 2136 West Mall, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z4, Canada E-mail: alec@psych.ubc.ca 1838 attraction, the well-documented presence of this ten- dency among men leads to an interesting prediction regarding women. Given women's brief fertile window, it may be adaptive for them to dress in a way that increases their sexual attractiveness to men during this period. Women thus may self-adorn in red or pink during this time as part of a broader drive to appear more sexually attractive at peak fertility (Durante, Li, & Haselton, 2008; Haselton & Gangestad, 2006). Alternatively, if the hor- mones associated with ovulation make women feel sexier during peak fertility (Haselton & Gangestad, 2006), they may be inclined to wear clothing with attention- grabbing properties (e.g., red-colored clothing) during this time, even in the absence of any desire to attract men. Despite these possibilities, prior research has sug- gested that the advertisement of human female fertility is largely hidden, involving vocal, olfactory, and subtle visual changes, but few overt behavioral displays (Bryant & Haselton, 2008; Miller & Maner, 2011). For example, although two studies have suggested that women's faces are judged as more attractive during peak fertility, the women in these studies posed neutral expressions and removed all cosmetics prior to being photographed, making it unlikely that their increased attractiveness was due to any objectively measureable behavioral display (Puts et al., 2013; Roberts et al., 2004). Other studies have documented changes in women's mate-seeking desires and behavioral tendencies during peak fertility: a self-reported increased desire to have sex with men (e.g., Haselton & Gangestad, 2006), an increased attraction to physical markers of health and masculinity (Penton-Voak & Perrett, 2000), an increased desire to wear revealing clothing (Durante et al., 2008), and a ten- dency to wear clothing that leads women to be judged as trying to look more attractive (Haselton et al., 2007). In addition, one study found that women at peak fertility wore more revealing clothing, but this effect emerged only among partnered women (whose partners were absent) attending Austrian discotheques, where, presum- ably, dressing provocatively does not violate social norms (Grammer, Renninger, & Fischer, 2004). These caveats are important because, despite several attempts, prior studies have thus far failed to document a generalized, objec- tively observable change in women's everyday behavior or dress that is linked to ovulation. Furthermore, although Haselton, Mortezaie, Pillsworth, Bleske-Rechek, and Frederick (2007) found that part- nered women were rated as dressing more fashionably at peak fertility, there was no difference in ratings of sexi- ness (and, again, the effect on fashionableness was restricted to partnered women). Similarly, although Durante and colleagues (2008; Durante, Griskevicius, Hill, Perilloux, & Li, 2011) found that women at peak fertility reported a desire to purchase and wear sexier Beall, Tracy clothing when imagining attending a social gathering at which they might meet men, no difference emerged in the observed sexiness of the clothing the women actually wore. Durante and colleagues (2008) argued that experi- mental studies may fail to document an ovulation-linked change in women's dress because dressing in provoca- tive clothing is often not socially acceptable, particularly for women participating in research on a university cam- pus. As a result, it remains unclear whether there is any salient, observable behavioral display reliably associated with female ovulation. Building on the evidence reviewed above suggesting that women may seek to increase their attractiveness by self-adorning in reddish colors, and should be particu- larly motivated to do so during peak fertility, we tested whether women are more likely to wear red- or pink- colored clothing during this period, compared with other phases of their menstrual cycle. Support for this predic- tion would provide the first evidence for a distinct and visually obvious behavioral display linked to female ovulation. Method Participants Sample A. A total of 100 American women (aged 18-47 years, Mdn = 26) recruited through Amazon's Mechanical Turk (see Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011) partici- pated in return for monetary compensation. Sample B. A total of 24 female undergraduates at the University of British Columbia (aged 17-31 years, Mdn = 19) participated in exchange for course credit. Procedure Two samples of regularly ovulating female participants completed measures online. Participants responded to the question, "What color is the shirt you are currently wearing? (If your shirt is multicolored, please select the color which is most prevalent)." Response options were "black," "blue," "gray," "green," "pink," "red," "white," "yel- low," and "other." Participants also responded to the question, "How many days has it been since the onset of your last period of menses? Responses were used to divide women into a high-conception-risk group (Days 6-14; Sample A: n = 51; Sample B: n = 10) and a low- conception-risk group (Days 0-5 and 15-28; Sample A n = 49, Sample B: n = 14) on the basis of a standard 28-day model of the menstrual cycle (Penton-Voak & Perrett, 2000). We excluded women whose first day of menses had occurred more than 28 days previously to avoid including women experiencing atypical cycles (Little, Jones, & DeBruine, 2008).²
Female
Fertility Cue
To address concerns regarding the assessment of
menstrual history via self-report (Bean, Leeper, Wallace,
Sherman, & Jagger, 1979; but see Baker, Denning, Kostin,
& Schwartz, 1998), we also asked participants, "Within
how many days are you 100% confident in your above
estimate?" Participants responded using a scale from 1 to
7 with the following anchors: 1 = 0 days (I'm 100% con-
fident), 2= 1 day, 3= 2 days, 4 = 3 days, 5= 4 days, 6 =
5 days, and 7 More than 5 days (I'm not very confi-
dent). We excluded all participants who responded with
"7" (n=9) and all for whom we could not determine
conception-risk-category membership with 100% cer-
tainty (n = 47). Specifically, we excluded, for example,
any participant who indicated that her last menses had
begun 12 days previously but was 100% confident of that
estimate within 3 days. In that case, we would assume
that her last period had begun within the past 9 to 15
days and, thus, she could not be included in either the
high-conception-risk group (Days 6-14) or the low-
conception-risk group (Days 0-5 and 15-28). In con-
trast, any participant who indicated that her last menses
had begun 10 days previously and was 100% confident
within 3 days would be included because we could
assume that her period had begun within the past 7 to 13
days, which would place her firmly within the high-
conception-risk group (Days 6-14).
Results
Women were classified as wearing a red or pink shirt
(Sample A, n = 17; Sample B, n = 5) or a different-colored
shirt (Sample A, n = 83; Sample B, n = 19). Women at
high conception risk were substantially more likely to be
wearing a red or pink shirt compared with women at low
conception risk-Sample A: 26% vs. 8%, x²(1, N= 100) =
5.32, p= .02, odds ratio= 3.85; Sample B: 40 % vs. 7%,
x(1, N=24)= 3.82, p= .051, odds ratio = 8.67. Examining
the likelihood that a woman's shirt-color choice predicted
ovulation, we found that 76% of women in Sample A and
80% of women in Sample B who were wearing red or
pink were at peak fertility, which suggests that reddish-
colored clothing is a strong indicator of ovulation (see
Fig. 1 for results). In contrast, only 46% of women in
Sample A and 32% of women in Sample B who were
wearing shirts of any other color were at high conception
risk. Indeed, conception risk had no effect on the preva-
lence of any other shirt color in either sample (for addi-
tional details, see the Supplemental Material available
online).
Discussion
Across two samples of women, those at high conception
risk were 3.5 times more likely to be wearing red or pink
Women at High Conception Risk
100%
90%
80%
70%
60%
50%
40%
40%
153046
30%
20%
10%
0%
Red- or Pink-Colored Shirt
Different-Colored Shirt
Community
1839
Undergraduate
Sample
Fig. 1. Percentage of women at high conception risk in the two sam-
ples as a function of shirt color. Error bars indicate standard errors of
the mean.
than were those at low risk, and those who wore red or
pink were more than 3 times as likely to be at high con-
ception risk than at low conception risk. These findings
support the prediction that displays of red and pink are a
reliable fertility cue in women and are the first to suggest
a visually salient, publicly observable, objective behavior
that is associated with female ovulation.
The underlying mechanism accounting for the present
findings is, as of yet, unknown, but it may be best
explained as a by-product of other psychological and
motivational changes that occur with ovulation. Past
research has suggested that women desire to dress sexier
during ovulation; however, studies have largely failed to
demonstrate any consistent behavioral change in the sex-
iness of women's dress across periods of conception risk
(Durante et al., 2008; Grammer et al., 2004; Haselton &
Gangestad, 2006; Haselton et al., 2007). The current
investigation offers a possible explanation for this dis-
crepancy: Although women at peak fertility may largely
refrain from dressing more provocatively out of social-
normative concerns (Durante et al., 2008), they may
nonetheless seek to increase their apparent sexiness by
self-adorning in the colors that are known to increase
their attractiveness to men and that, at least in North
American contexts, are not associated with any social
stigma. Future studies are needed to test whether the
present results were due to an increased desire among
ovulating women to dress in a more sexually attractive
manner. It is noteworthy, in this context, that prior stud-
ies examining male sexual interest have found that men's
attraction to women wearing red holds when controlling
for the sexiness of the women's clothing (as well as other
relevant variables; Elliot & Niesta, 2008), raising the
1840
possibility that the present findings also may have been
due to something about redness that cannot be attributed
entirely to ovulating women's desire to dress sexier.
Regardless of this issue, the present findings are likely
to be linked, in some manner, to men's tendency to find
red and pink attractive. An important question, then, is
why are men, cross-culturally, so attracted to these
colors? Several researchers have suggested that this pro-
clivity is the result of an adaptation originating in our non-
human primate ancestors, due to the fact that in some
primate species (e.g., chimpanzees), females' genitals
acquire an extreme red or pink coloration during ovula-
tion (i.e., estrous swellings) from increased vascularization
(Elliot & Niesta, 2008; Gerald, 2003). The visibility of these
swellings would make it adaptive for males of these spe-
cies to find redness in females attractive, and it is possible
that a cognitive mechanism associating red with attractive-
ness thus emerged in a shared nonhuman ancestor and
was retained in humans. This account presupposes that
estrous swellings were present in at least some ancestor
shared between humans and these other primates; it is
thus challenged by evidence that exaggerated swellings
emerged only after the chimpanzee genus diverged from
the line that led to modern humans (Pagel & Meade, 2006).
However, other researchers have suggested that human
ancestors may have displayed some form of considerably
subtler visual signs of estrus, including a slight reddening
of the anogenital area (Sillen-Tullberg & Moller, 1993), and
that humans' erect posture concealed this coloration such
that, eventually, female fertility signaling through red col-
oration became maladaptive because of the energy
expended creating such displays (see Pawlowski, 1999).
Given these competing accounts, this is an issue that
would benefit from future investigations.
Another possibility is that red came to be associated
with sexuality because of its impact on sensory systems
unrelated to mating (Kirkpatrick & Ryan, 1991). Ancestral
foraging practices made spotting ripened fruit (indicated
through redness) paramount to survival (Osorio &
Vorobyev, 1996), so the human perceptual system might
have become finely attuned to this color, and men's ten-
dency to find it attractive could be a by-product of red's
broader attention-grabbing properties. Future research is
needed to examine whether the present effects were due
to an increased desire among women to attract visual
attention during the period when they feel sexier, even in
the absence of any desire to attract men (and, conversely,
whether the effects were due to women at low fertility
feeling less sexy and therefore refraining from wearing
such clothing).
One methodological limitation of the present research
was our reliance on self-report rather than hormonal
measures to assess conception risk. Although con-
cerns have been raised regarding the reliability of the
Beall, Tracy
assessment method we used (Bean et al., 1979; but see
Baker et al., 1998), our invocation of a new method to
assess women's confidence in their self-reported menses
onset may be of use in future research. By taking into
account women's uncertainty on this issue, this method
ameliorates some concerns regarding the use of self-
reported estimates. Furthermore, if errors were made in
our categorization of women into high- and low-concep-
tion-risk groups, these errors would reduce our power to
find effects, making the reported effects conservative
estimates of actual effect sizes. Nonetheless, future stud-
ies should seek to replicate and extend our findings
using hormonal assessment techniques.
Regardless of these issues, which highlight important
directions for future research, the present results are the
first to indicate that female ovulation, long assumed to be
hidden, is in fact associated with a distinct, objectively
observable behavioral display.
Author Contributions
Both authors contributed to the study design. Data collection,
analyses, and interpretations were performed by A. T. Beall
under the supervision of J. L Tracy. Both authors contributed
to the composition of the manuscript, with A. T. Beall compos-
ing initial drafts. Both authors approved the final version of the
manuscript for submission.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
Supplemental Material
Additional supporting information may be found at http://pss
sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data
Notes
1. We requested that women not participate if they were more
than 40 years old, users of hormonal birth control, cigarette
smokers, pregnant, or not experiencing regular menstrual
cycles (see Haselton & Gildersleeve, 2011). We also requested
that women not participate if they were within 5 days of the
onset of menses, to minimize the inclusion of women for whom
effects might be attributable to menstrual or premenstrual.
symptoms (see Haselton & Gildersleeve, 2011). However, 22%
of participants in Sample A and 38% of participants in Sample
B reported being within this 10-day window and were included
in all analyses (all reported results across the two samples
held when these women were excluded). It is noteworthy that
these recruitment efforts and exclusion criteria resulted in a
large proportion of women in the high-conception-risk cate-
gory (Sample A: 51%; Sample B: 42%).
2. Results held, collapsing across the two samples, when these
women (n=9) were included in analyses.
3. Results held, collapsing across the two samples, when these
women (n=47) were included in analyses.
Transcribed Image Text:Female Fertility Cue To address concerns regarding the assessment of menstrual history via self-report (Bean, Leeper, Wallace, Sherman, & Jagger, 1979; but see Baker, Denning, Kostin, & Schwartz, 1998), we also asked participants, "Within how many days are you 100% confident in your above estimate?" Participants responded using a scale from 1 to 7 with the following anchors: 1 = 0 days (I'm 100% con- fident), 2= 1 day, 3= 2 days, 4 = 3 days, 5= 4 days, 6 = 5 days, and 7 More than 5 days (I'm not very confi- dent). We excluded all participants who responded with "7" (n=9) and all for whom we could not determine conception-risk-category membership with 100% cer- tainty (n = 47). Specifically, we excluded, for example, any participant who indicated that her last menses had begun 12 days previously but was 100% confident of that estimate within 3 days. In that case, we would assume that her last period had begun within the past 9 to 15 days and, thus, she could not be included in either the high-conception-risk group (Days 6-14) or the low- conception-risk group (Days 0-5 and 15-28). In con- trast, any participant who indicated that her last menses had begun 10 days previously and was 100% confident within 3 days would be included because we could assume that her period had begun within the past 7 to 13 days, which would place her firmly within the high- conception-risk group (Days 6-14). Results Women were classified as wearing a red or pink shirt (Sample A, n = 17; Sample B, n = 5) or a different-colored shirt (Sample A, n = 83; Sample B, n = 19). Women at high conception risk were substantially more likely to be wearing a red or pink shirt compared with women at low conception risk-Sample A: 26% vs. 8%, x²(1, N= 100) = 5.32, p= .02, odds ratio= 3.85; Sample B: 40 % vs. 7%, x(1, N=24)= 3.82, p= .051, odds ratio = 8.67. Examining the likelihood that a woman's shirt-color choice predicted ovulation, we found that 76% of women in Sample A and 80% of women in Sample B who were wearing red or pink were at peak fertility, which suggests that reddish- colored clothing is a strong indicator of ovulation (see Fig. 1 for results). In contrast, only 46% of women in Sample A and 32% of women in Sample B who were wearing shirts of any other color were at high conception risk. Indeed, conception risk had no effect on the preva- lence of any other shirt color in either sample (for addi- tional details, see the Supplemental Material available online). Discussion Across two samples of women, those at high conception risk were 3.5 times more likely to be wearing red or pink Women at High Conception Risk 100% 90% 80% 70% 60% 50% 40% 40% 153046 30% 20% 10% 0% Red- or Pink-Colored Shirt Different-Colored Shirt Community 1839 Undergraduate Sample Fig. 1. Percentage of women at high conception risk in the two sam- ples as a function of shirt color. Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean. than were those at low risk, and those who wore red or pink were more than 3 times as likely to be at high con- ception risk than at low conception risk. These findings support the prediction that displays of red and pink are a reliable fertility cue in women and are the first to suggest a visually salient, publicly observable, objective behavior that is associated with female ovulation. The underlying mechanism accounting for the present findings is, as of yet, unknown, but it may be best explained as a by-product of other psychological and motivational changes that occur with ovulation. Past research has suggested that women desire to dress sexier during ovulation; however, studies have largely failed to demonstrate any consistent behavioral change in the sex- iness of women's dress across periods of conception risk (Durante et al., 2008; Grammer et al., 2004; Haselton & Gangestad, 2006; Haselton et al., 2007). The current investigation offers a possible explanation for this dis- crepancy: Although women at peak fertility may largely refrain from dressing more provocatively out of social- normative concerns (Durante et al., 2008), they may nonetheless seek to increase their apparent sexiness by self-adorning in the colors that are known to increase their attractiveness to men and that, at least in North American contexts, are not associated with any social stigma. Future studies are needed to test whether the present results were due to an increased desire among ovulating women to dress in a more sexually attractive manner. It is noteworthy, in this context, that prior stud- ies examining male sexual interest have found that men's attraction to women wearing red holds when controlling for the sexiness of the women's clothing (as well as other relevant variables; Elliot & Niesta, 2008), raising the 1840 possibility that the present findings also may have been due to something about redness that cannot be attributed entirely to ovulating women's desire to dress sexier. Regardless of this issue, the present findings are likely to be linked, in some manner, to men's tendency to find red and pink attractive. An important question, then, is why are men, cross-culturally, so attracted to these colors? Several researchers have suggested that this pro- clivity is the result of an adaptation originating in our non- human primate ancestors, due to the fact that in some primate species (e.g., chimpanzees), females' genitals acquire an extreme red or pink coloration during ovula- tion (i.e., estrous swellings) from increased vascularization (Elliot & Niesta, 2008; Gerald, 2003). The visibility of these swellings would make it adaptive for males of these spe- cies to find redness in females attractive, and it is possible that a cognitive mechanism associating red with attractive- ness thus emerged in a shared nonhuman ancestor and was retained in humans. This account presupposes that estrous swellings were present in at least some ancestor shared between humans and these other primates; it is thus challenged by evidence that exaggerated swellings emerged only after the chimpanzee genus diverged from the line that led to modern humans (Pagel & Meade, 2006). However, other researchers have suggested that human ancestors may have displayed some form of considerably subtler visual signs of estrus, including a slight reddening of the anogenital area (Sillen-Tullberg & Moller, 1993), and that humans' erect posture concealed this coloration such that, eventually, female fertility signaling through red col- oration became maladaptive because of the energy expended creating such displays (see Pawlowski, 1999). Given these competing accounts, this is an issue that would benefit from future investigations. Another possibility is that red came to be associated with sexuality because of its impact on sensory systems unrelated to mating (Kirkpatrick & Ryan, 1991). Ancestral foraging practices made spotting ripened fruit (indicated through redness) paramount to survival (Osorio & Vorobyev, 1996), so the human perceptual system might have become finely attuned to this color, and men's ten- dency to find it attractive could be a by-product of red's broader attention-grabbing properties. Future research is needed to examine whether the present effects were due to an increased desire among women to attract visual attention during the period when they feel sexier, even in the absence of any desire to attract men (and, conversely, whether the effects were due to women at low fertility feeling less sexy and therefore refraining from wearing such clothing). One methodological limitation of the present research was our reliance on self-report rather than hormonal measures to assess conception risk. Although con- cerns have been raised regarding the reliability of the Beall, Tracy assessment method we used (Bean et al., 1979; but see Baker et al., 1998), our invocation of a new method to assess women's confidence in their self-reported menses onset may be of use in future research. By taking into account women's uncertainty on this issue, this method ameliorates some concerns regarding the use of self- reported estimates. Furthermore, if errors were made in our categorization of women into high- and low-concep- tion-risk groups, these errors would reduce our power to find effects, making the reported effects conservative estimates of actual effect sizes. Nonetheless, future stud- ies should seek to replicate and extend our findings using hormonal assessment techniques. Regardless of these issues, which highlight important directions for future research, the present results are the first to indicate that female ovulation, long assumed to be hidden, is in fact associated with a distinct, objectively observable behavioral display. Author Contributions Both authors contributed to the study design. Data collection, analyses, and interpretations were performed by A. T. Beall under the supervision of J. L Tracy. Both authors contributed to the composition of the manuscript, with A. T. Beall compos- ing initial drafts. Both authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article. Supplemental Material Additional supporting information may be found at http://pss sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data Notes 1. We requested that women not participate if they were more than 40 years old, users of hormonal birth control, cigarette smokers, pregnant, or not experiencing regular menstrual cycles (see Haselton & Gildersleeve, 2011). We also requested that women not participate if they were within 5 days of the onset of menses, to minimize the inclusion of women for whom effects might be attributable to menstrual or premenstrual. symptoms (see Haselton & Gildersleeve, 2011). However, 22% of participants in Sample A and 38% of participants in Sample B reported being within this 10-day window and were included in all analyses (all reported results across the two samples held when these women were excluded). It is noteworthy that these recruitment efforts and exclusion criteria resulted in a large proportion of women in the high-conception-risk cate- gory (Sample A: 51%; Sample B: 42%). 2. Results held, collapsing across the two samples, when these women (n=9) were included in analyses. 3. Results held, collapsing across the two samples, when these women (n=47) were included in analyses.
Expert Solution
steps

Step by step

Solved in 4 steps

Blurred answer
Recommended textbooks for you
Ciccarelli: Psychology_5 (5th Edition)
Ciccarelli: Psychology_5 (5th Edition)
Psychology
ISBN:
9780134477961
Author:
Saundra K. Ciccarelli, J. Noland White
Publisher:
PEARSON
Cognitive Psychology
Cognitive Psychology
Psychology
ISBN:
9781337408271
Author:
Goldstein, E. Bruce.
Publisher:
Cengage Learning,
Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and …
Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and …
Psychology
ISBN:
9781337565691
Author:
Dennis Coon, John O. Mitterer, Tanya S. Martini
Publisher:
Cengage Learning
Psychology in Your Life (Second Edition)
Psychology in Your Life (Second Edition)
Psychology
ISBN:
9780393265156
Author:
Sarah Grison, Michael Gazzaniga
Publisher:
W. W. Norton & Company
Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research a…
Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research a…
Psychology
ISBN:
9781285763880
Author:
E. Bruce Goldstein
Publisher:
Cengage Learning
Theories of Personality (MindTap Course List)
Theories of Personality (MindTap Course List)
Psychology
ISBN:
9781305652958
Author:
Duane P. Schultz, Sydney Ellen Schultz
Publisher:
Cengage Learning